Show simple item record

A Matter of Perspective: Choosing for Others Differs from Choosing for Yourself in Making Treatment Decisions

dc.contributor.authorZikmund-Fisher, Brian J.en_US
dc.contributor.authorSarr, Briannaen_US
dc.contributor.authorFagerlin, Angelaen_US
dc.contributor.authorUbel, Peter A.en_US
dc.date.accessioned2010-06-01T19:14:02Z
dc.date.available2010-06-01T19:14:02Z
dc.date.issued2006-06en_US
dc.identifier.citationZikmund-Fisher, Brian J . ; Sarr, Brianna; Fagerlin, Angela; Ubel, Peter A . (2006). "A Matter of Perspective: Choosing for Others Differs from Choosing for Yourself in Making Treatment Decisions." Journal of General Internal Medicine 21(6): 618-622. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/72417>en_US
dc.identifier.issn0884-8734en_US
dc.identifier.issn1525-1497en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/72417
dc.identifier.urihttp://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/sites/entrez?cmd=retrieve&db=pubmed&list_uids=16808746&dopt=citationen_US
dc.description.abstractMany people display omission bias in medical decision making, accepting the risk of passive nonintervention rather than actively choosing interventions (such as vaccinations) that result in lower levels of risk. OBJECTIVE : Testing whether people's preferences for active interventions would increase when deciding for others versus for themselves. RESEARCH DESIGN : Survey participants imagined themselves in 1 of 4 roles: patient, physician treating a single patient, medical director creating treatment guidelines, or parent deciding for a child. All read 2 short scenarios about vaccinations for a deadly flu and treatments for a slow-growing cancer. PARTICIPANTS : Two thousand three hundred and ninety-nine people drawn from a demographically stratified internet sample. MEASURES : Chosen or recommended treatments. We also measured participants' emotional response to our task. RESULTS : Preferences for risk-reducing active treatments were significantly stronger for participants imagining themselves as medical professionals than for those imagining themselves as patients (vaccination: 73% [physician] & 63% [medical director] vs 48% [patient], P s<.001; chemotherapy: 68% & 68% vs 60%, P s<.012). Similar results were observed for the parental role (vaccination: 57% vs 48%, P =.003; chemotherapy: 72% vs 60%, P <.001). Reported emotional reactions were stronger in the responsible medical professional and parental roles yet were also independently associated with treatment choice, with higher scores associated with reduced omission tendencies (OR=1.15 for both regressions, P s<.01). CONCLUSIONS : Treatment preferences may be substantially influenced by a decision-making role. As certain roles appear to reinforce “big picture” thinking about difficult risk tradeoffs, physicians and patients should consider re-framing treatment decisions to gain new, and hopefully beneficial, perspectives.en_US
dc.format.extent100372 bytes
dc.format.extent3109 bytes
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.format.mimetypetext/plain
dc.publisherBlackwell Publishing Incen_US
dc.rights© 2006 by the Society of General Internal Medicine. All rights reserveden_US
dc.subject.otherDecision Makingen_US
dc.subject.otherRisk Communicationen_US
dc.subject.otherOmission Biasen_US
dc.titleA Matter of Perspective: Choosing for Others Differs from Choosing for Yourself in Making Treatment Decisionsen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelInternal Medicine and Specialtiesen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelHealth Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumDivision of General Internal Medicine, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA ;en_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumDepartment of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA.en_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherVA Health Services Research & Development Center for Practice Management and Outcomes Research, VA Ann Arbor Healthcare System, Ann Arbor, MI, USA ;en_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherCenter for Behavioral and Decision Sciences in Medicine, Ann Arbor, MI, USA ;en_US
dc.identifier.pmid16808746en_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/72417/1/j.1525-1497.2006.00410.x.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.00410.xen_US
dc.identifier.sourceJournal of General Internal Medicineen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRitov I, Baron J. Reluctance to vaccinate: omission bias and ambiguity. J Behav Decis Making. 1990; 3: 263 – 77.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSpranca M, Minsk E, Baron J. Omission and commission in judgment and choice. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1991; 27: 76 – 105.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAsch DA, Baron J, Hershey JC, et al. Omission bias and pertussis vaccination. Med Decis Making. 1994; 14: 118 – 23.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMeszaros JR, Asch DA, Baron J, Hershey JC, Kunreuther H, Schwartz-Buzaglo J. Cognitive processes and the decisions of some parents to forego pertussis vaccination for their children. J Clin Epidemiol. 1996; 49: 697 – 703.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBaron J, Ritov I. Omission bias, individual differences, and normality. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2004; 94: 74 – 85.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHaidt J, Baron J. Social roles and the moral judgement of acts and omissions. Eur J Soc Psychol. 1996; 26: 201 – 18.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLoewenstein GF, Weber EU, Hsee CK, Welch N. Risk as feelings. Psychol Bull. 2001; 127: 267 – 86.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKray L, Gonzalez R. Differential weighting in choice versus advice: I'll do this, you do that. J Behav Decis Making. 1999; 12: 207 – 17.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJonas E, Schulz-Hardt S, Frey D. Giving advice or making decisions in someone else's place: the influence of impression, defense, and accuracy motivation on the search for new information. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2005; 31: 977 – 90.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDemoratz MJ. Advance directives: getting patients to complete them before they need them. Case Manager. 2005; 16: 61 – 3.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLantos J. Informed consent. The whole truth for patients?. Cancer. 1993; 72 ( suppl 9 ): 2811 – 5.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKurzon N. A poker player's guide to beating cancer. Newsweek 2004, January 19, 2004:12.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAsch DA, Hershey JC. Why some health policies don't make sense at the bedside. Ann Intern Med. 1995; 122: 846 – 50.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChant AD. Practising doctors should not manage. Lancet. 1984; 1: 1398.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWroe AL, Turner N, Salkovskis PM. Understanding and predicting parental decisions about early childhood immunizations. Health Psychol. 2004; 23: 33 – 41.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWroe AL, Bhan A, Salkovskis P, Bedford H. Feeling bad about immunising our children. Vaccine. 2005; 23: 1428 – 33.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWatson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988; 54: 1063 – 70.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarstensen LL, Isaacowitz DM, Charles CT. Taking time seriously: a theory of socioemotional selectivity. Am Psychol. 1999; 54: 165 – 81.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCharles S, Mather M, Carstensen LL. Aging and emotional memory: the forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2003; 132: 310 – 24.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePeters E, Finucane ML, MacGregor DG, Slovic P. The bearable lightness of aging: judgment and decision processes in older adults. In: Stern PC, Carstensen LL, eds. The Aging Mind: Opportunities in Cognitive Research. Washington, DC: National Research Council, National Academy Press; 2000: 144 – 65.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWilliams P, Drolet A. Age-related differences in responses to emotional advertisements. J Consum Res. 2005; 32: 343 – 54.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKray LJ. Contingent weighting in self-other decision making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2000; 83: 82 – 106.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceConnolly T, Zeelenberg M. Regret in decision making. Psychol Sci. 2004; 11: 212 – 6.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRaymark P. Accepting or rejecting medical treatment: a comparison of decisions made for self versus those made for a significant other. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2000; 30: 2409 – 36.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceUbel PA. “What should I do, doc?”: some psychologic benefits of physician recommendations. Arch Intern Med. 2002; 162: 977 – 80.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGurmankin AD, Baron J, Hershey JC, Ubel PA. The role of physicians recommendations in medical treatment decisions. Med Decis Making. 2002; 22: 262 – 71.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMcNutt RA. Shared medical decision making: problems, process, progress. JAMA. 2004; 292: 2516 – 8.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchneider CE. The Practice of Autonomy: Patients, Doctors, and Medical Decisions. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1998.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.