Show simple item record

Physiological Correlates of Choice‐Induced Dissonance: An Exploration of HPA‐Axis Responses

dc.contributor.authorKimel, Sasha Y.en_US
dc.contributor.authorLopez‐duran, Nestoren_US
dc.contributor.authorKitayama, Shinobuen_US
dc.date.accessioned2015-10-07T20:42:53Z
dc.date.available2016-12-01T14:33:06Zen
dc.date.issued2015-10en_US
dc.identifier.citationKimel, Sasha Y.; Lopez‐duran, Nestor ; Kitayama, Shinobu (2015). "Physiological Correlates of Choiceâ Induced Dissonance: An Exploration of HPAâ Axis Responses." Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 28(4): 309-316.en_US
dc.identifier.issn0894-3257en_US
dc.identifier.issn1099-0771en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/113729
dc.description.abstractChoice can produce a negatively arousing cognitive conflict (called dissonance), which is thought to motivate the chooser to spread their preferences for the relevant options (called Spreading of Alternatives, or SA). The current work aimed to determine the relationship between HPA‐axis activity and both choice‐induced dissonance and its reduction (i.e. SA) among individuals with varying cultural backgrounds. European–Americans and Asians made a choice between two equally attractive CDs either in the presence of a cue indicative of social eyes (i.e. public‐choice condition) or in the absence thereof (i.e. private‐choice condition). As predicted, European–Americans and Asians showed a reliable SA primarily in the private and public choice conditions, respectively. Importantly, a sharp decline of salivary cortisol was observed over the span of 30 min, and, moreover, this decline was reliably predicted by the magnitude of SA regardless of either culture or the choice being private vs. public. These results suggest that although choice‐induced dissonance is too weak to elicit an HPA‐axis stress response, SA is associated with variability in the decline of salivary cortisol during the laboratory task. Copyright © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.en_US
dc.publisherWiley Periodicals, Inc.en_US
dc.publisherAcademic Pressen_US
dc.subject.otherdecision makingen_US
dc.subject.othercognitive dissonanceen_US
dc.subject.othersocial eyesen_US
dc.subject.othercultureen_US
dc.subject.otherspreading of alternatives (SA)en_US
dc.subject.othercortisolen_US
dc.subject.otherhypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axisen_US
dc.subject.otherchoiceen_US
dc.titlePhysiological Correlates of Choice‐Induced Dissonance: An Exploration of HPA‐Axis Responsesen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollowen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPsychologyen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/113729/1/bdm1845.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1002/bdm.1845en_US
dc.identifier.sourceJournal of Behavioral Decision Makingen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMason, J. W. ( 1968 ). A review of psychoendocrine research on the pituitary–adrenal corticalsystem. Psychosomatic Medicine, 30, 576 – 607.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHoshino‐Browne, E., Zanna, A. S., Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., Kitayama, S., & Lackenbauer, S. ( 2005 ). On the cultural guises of cognitive dissonance: The case of easterners and westerners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 294 – 310.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHruschka, D. J., Kohrt, B. A., & Worthman, C. M. ( 2005 ). Estimating between‐ and within individual variation in cortisol levels using multilevel models. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30 ( 7 ), 698 – 714en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceImada, T., & Kitayama, S. ( 2010 ). Social eyes and choice justification: Culture and dissonance revisited. Social Cognition, 28 ( 5 ), 589 – 608.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKimel, S. Y., Grossmann, I., & Kitayama S. ( 2012 ). When gift‐giving produces dissonance: Effects of subliminal affiliation priming on choices for one's self versus close others. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48 ( 5 ), 1221 – 1224.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKitayama, S., Chua, H. F., Tompson, S., & Han, S. ( 2013 ). Neural mechanisms of dissonance: An fMRI investigation of choice justification. NeuroImage, 13, 206 – 212.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKitayama, S., Snibbe, A. C., Markus, H. R., & Suzuki, T. ( 2004 ). Is there any " free" choice? self and dissonance in two cultures. Psychological Science, 15 ( 8 ), 527 – 533.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKitayama, S., Tompson, S., & Chua, H. F. ( 2014 ). Cultural neuroscience of choice justification. In J. Forgas, & E. Harmon‐Jones (Eds.), Control within: Motivation and its regulation (pp. 313 – 330 ). Sydney Symposium: Psychology Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKudielka, B. M., Schommer, N. C., Hellhammer D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. ( 2004 ). Acute HPA axis responses, heart rate, and mood changes to psychosocial stress (TSST) in humans at different times of day. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 983 – 992.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLosch, M. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. ( 1990 ). Cognitive dissonance may enhance sympathetic tonus, but attitudes are changed to reduce negative affect rather than arousal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 289 – 304.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLovallo, W. R., & Thomas, T. L. ( 2000 ). Stress hormones in psychophysiological research: emotional, behavioral, and cognitive implications. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G.G. Bernston (Eds.) Handbook of psychophysiology ( Second ed., pp. 342 – 367 ). New York: Cambridge University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMarkus H. R., & Kitayama, S. ( 1991 ). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224 – 253.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMatthews, J. N., Altman, D. G., Campbell, M. J., & Royston, P. ( 1990 ). Analysis of serial measurements in medical research. British Medical Journal, 300 ( 6719 ), 230 – 235.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNa, J., & Kitayama, S. ( 2012 ). Will people work hard on a task they choose? Social‐eyes priming in different cultural contexts. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48 ( 1 ), 284 – 290.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePark, J., & Kitayama, S. ( 2014 ). Interdependent selves show face‐induced facilitation of error processing: Cultural neuroscience of self‐threat. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9 ( 2 ), 201 – 208.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRanjit, N., Young, E. A., Raghunathan, T. E., & Kaplan, G. A. ( 2005 ). Modeling cortisol rhythms in a population‐based study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 615 – 624.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRigdon, M., Ishii, K., Watabe, M., & Kitayama, S. ( 2009 ). Minimal social cues in the dictator game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30, 358 – 367.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchultheiss, O. C., & Stanton, S. J. ( 2009 ). Assessment of salivary hormones. In E. Harmon‐Jones, & J. S. Beer (Eds.), Methods in the neurobiology of social and personality psychology (pp. 17 – 44 ). New York, NY: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchwabe, L., Haddad, L., & Schachinger, H. ( 2008 ). HPA axis activation by a socially evaluated cold‐pressor test. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33, 890 – 895.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceShirtcliff, E. A., Granger, D. A., Schwartz, E., Curran, M. J. ( 2001 ). Use of salivary biomarkers in bio‐behavioral research: Cotton‐based sample collection methods can interfere with salivary immunoassay results. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26 ( 2 ), 165 – 173.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSteele, C. M. ( 1988 ). The psychology of self‐affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 21 ( 2 ), 261 – 302.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSteele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Lynch, M. ( 1993 ). Self‐image resilience and dissonance: The role of affirmational resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64 ( 6 ), 885 – 896.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTsigos, C., & Chrousos, G. P., ( 2002 ). Hypothalamic‐pituitary‐adrenal axis, neuroendocrine factors and stress. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53 ( 4 ), 865 – 871.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceYim, I. S., Quas, J. A., Cahill, L., & Hayakawa, C. M. ( 2010 ). Children's and adults' salivary cortisol responses to an identical psychosocial laboratory stressor. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 241 – 248.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceYoung, E. A., Abelson, J. L., & Lightman, S. L. ( 2004 ). Cortisol pulsatility and its role in stress regulation and health. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 25, 69 – 76.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAbelson, J. L., Erickson, T. M., Mayer, S. E., Crocker, J., Briggs, H., Lopez‐Duran, N. L., & Liberzon, I. ( 2014 ). Brief cognitive intervention can modulate neuroendocrine stress responses to the trier social stress test: Buffering effects of a compassionate goal orientation. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 44, 60 – 70. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.02.016en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBjorklund, A., Hokfelt, T., & Owman, C., ( 2010 ). Sympathetic nervous system. In George Fink (Ed.), Stress science: neuroendocrinology (Vol. 47, pp. 290 ). Oxford, UK: Academic Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBrehm, J. W. ( 2007 ). A brief history of dissonance theory. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 1 ( 1 ), 381 – 391.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBrehm, J. W. ( 1956 ). Post‐decision changes in the desirability of alternatives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52 ( 2 ), 384 – 389.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChen, M. K., & Risen, J. L. ( 2010 ). How choice affects and reflects preferences: Revisiting the free‐choice paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99 ( 4 ), 573 – 594.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCooper, J., & Fazio, R. ( 1984 ). A new look at dissonance theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 229 – 264.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCroyle, R. T., & Cooper, J. ( 1983 ). Dissonance arousal: Physiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 782 – 791.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCurley, S. P., Yates, J. F., & Abrams, R. A. ( 1986 ). Psychological sources of ambiguity avoidance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 38 ( 2 ), 230 – 256.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDabbs, J. M. ( 1991 ). Salivary testosterone measurement: Collecting, storing and mailing salivary samples. Physiology & Behavior, 49, 815 – 817.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. ( 2004 ). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin, 130 ( 3 ), 355 – 391.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEdwards, S., Clow, A., Evans, P., & Hucklebridge, F. ( 2001 ). Exploration of the awakening cortisol response in relation to diurnal cortisol secretory activity. Life Sciences, 68 ( 18 ), 2093 – 2103.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceElkin, R. A., & Leippe, M. R. ( 1986 ). Physiological arousal, dissonance, and attitude change: Evidence for a dissonance‐arousal link and a " don't remind me" effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 55 – 65.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceElliot, A. J., & Devine, P. G. ( 1994 ). On the motivation nature of cognitive dissonance: Dissonance as psychological discomfort. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 382 – 394.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFestinger, L. ( 1957 ). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGerard, H. B. ( 1967 ). Choice difficulty, dissonance, and the decision sequence. Journal of Personality, 35, 91 – 108.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGueorguieva, R. ( 2004 ). Move over ANOVA. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61 ( 3 ), 310.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHaley, K. J., & Fessler, D. M. T. ( 2005 ). Nobody's watching? Evolution and Human Behavior, 26 ( 3 ), 245 – 256. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.01.002en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHarmon‐Jones, E., Amodio, D. M., & Harmon‐Jones, C. ( 2009 ). Action‐based model of dissonance: A review, integration, and expansion of conceptions of cognitive conflict. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 119 – 166.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHarmon‐Jones, E., & Harmon‐Jones, C. ( 2009 ). Action‐based model of dissonance: A review of behavioral, anterior cingulate, and prefrontal cortical mechanisms. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2 ( 3 ), 1518 – 1538.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHeine, S. J., & Lehman, D. R. ( 1997 ). The cultural construction of self‐enhancement: an examination of group‐serving biases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72 ( 6 ), 1268.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHiggins, E. T., Rhodewalt, F., & Zanna, M. P. ( 1979 ). Dissonance motivation: Its nature, persistence, and reinstatement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 15, 16 – 34.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.