Bilingualism alters children’s frontal lobe functioning for attentional control
dc.contributor.author | Arredondo, Maria M. | |
dc.contributor.author | Hu, Xiao‐su | |
dc.contributor.author | Satterfield, Teresa | |
dc.contributor.author | Kovelman, Ioulia | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2017-05-10T17:47:59Z | |
dc.date.available | 2018-07-09T17:42:24Z | en |
dc.date.issued | 2017-05 | |
dc.identifier.citation | Arredondo, Maria M.; Hu, Xiao‐su ; Satterfield, Teresa; Kovelman, Ioulia (2017). "Bilingualism alters children’s frontal lobe functioning for attentional control." Developmental Science 20(3): n/a-n/a. | |
dc.identifier.issn | 1363-755X | |
dc.identifier.issn | 1467-7687 | |
dc.identifier.uri | https://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/136694 | |
dc.description.abstract | Bilingualism is a typical linguistic experience, yet relatively little is known about its impact on children’s cognitive and brain development. Theories of bilingualism suggest that early dualâ language acquisition can improve children’s cognitive abilities, specifically those relying on frontal lobe functioning. While behavioral findings present much conflicting evidence, little is known about its effects on children’s frontal lobe development. Using functional nearâ infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), the findings suggest that Spanishâ English bilingual children (n = 13, ages 7â 13) had greater activation in left prefrontal cortex during a nonâ verbal attentional control task relative to ageâ matched English monolinguals. In contrast, monolinguals (n = 14) showed greater right prefrontal activation than bilinguals. The present findings suggest that early bilingualism yields significant changes to the functional organization of children’s prefrontal cortex for attentional control and carry implications for understanding how early life experiences impact cognition and brain development.This fNIRS study investigated the impact of bilingual exposure on children’s brain organization for attentional control (N = 27, ages 7â 13). During a nonâ verbal attention task, bilinguals showed greater left frontal lobe activation than monolinguals. Monolinguals showed greater right frontal lobe activation than bilinguals. The findings suggest that bilingualism affects the functionality of children’s left prefrontal cortex for attentional control. | |
dc.publisher | Academic Therapy Publications | |
dc.publisher | Wiley Periodicals, Inc. | |
dc.title | Bilingualism alters children’s frontal lobe functioning for attentional control | |
dc.type | Article | en_US |
dc.rights.robots | IndexNoFollow | |
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevel | Pediatrics | |
dc.subject.hlbtoplevel | Health Sciences | |
dc.description.peerreviewed | Peer Reviewed | |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/136694/1/desc12377-sup-0001-FigS1.pdf | |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/136694/2/desc12377.pdf | |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/136694/3/desc12377_am.pdf | |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/136694/4/desc12377-sup-0003-SupInfo.pdf | |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1111/desc.12377 | |
dc.identifier.source | Developmental Science | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Paap, K.R., & Greenberg, Z.I. ( 2013 ). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232 â 258. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Posner, M.I. ( 2012 ). Attention in a social world. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ponitz, C.C., McClelland, M.M., Matthews, J.S., & Morrison, F.J. ( 2009 ). Touch your knees! Using a direct observation of behavioral regulation to predict math, literacy, and vocabulary achievement in kindergarten. Developmental Psychology, 45, 605 â 619. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rodriguezâ Fornells, A., Rotte, M., Heinze, H.â J., Nösselt, T.M., & Münte, T.F. ( 2002 ). Brain potential and functional MRI evidence for how to handle two languages with one brain. Nature, 415, 1026 â 1029. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rorabacher, D.B. ( 1991 ). Statistical treatment for rejection of deviant values: critical values of Dixon Q parameter and related subrange ratios at the 95 percent confidence level. Analytical Chemistry, 63, 139 â 146. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rueda, M.R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B.D., Halparin, J.D., Gruber, D.B. et al. ( 2004 ). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1029 â 1040. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Semel, E., Wiig, E.H., & Secord, W.A. ( 2003 ). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Fourth Edition (CELFâ 4). Bloomington, MN: Pearson. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Semel, E., Wiig, E.H., & Secord, W.A. ( 2006 ). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Fourth Spanish Edition (CELFâ 4). Bloomington, MN: Pearson. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Singh, L., Fu, C.S.L., Rahman, A.A., Hameed, W.B., Sanmugam, S. et al. ( 2015 ). Back to basics: a bilingual advantage in infant visual habituation. Child Development, 86, 294 â 302. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Singh, A.K., & Dan, I. ( 2006 ). Exploring the false discovery rate in multichannel NIRS. NeuroImage, 33, 542 â 549. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Tran, C.D., Arredondo, M.M., & Yoshida, H. ( 2015 ). Differential effects of bilingualism and culture on early attention: a longitudinal study in the US, Argentina, and Vietnam. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 795. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00795 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Valian, V. ( 2015 ). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 3 â 24. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Van Hell, J., & Dijkstra, T. ( 2002 ). Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 780 â 789. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wager, T.D., Sylvester, C.â Y.C., Lacey, S.C., Nee, D.E., Franklin, M. et al. ( 2005 ). Common and unique components of response inhibition revealed by fMRI. NeuroImage, 27, 323 â 340. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wagner, R.K., Torgesen, J.K., & Rashotte, C.A. ( 1999 ). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP). Austin, TX: Proâ Ed. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wolf, M., & Denckla, M. ( 2005 ). The Rapid Automatized Naming and Rapid Alternating Stimulus Tests (RAN/RAS). Austin, TX: Proâ Ed. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Woodcock, R.W. ( 1998 ). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests â Revised Normative Update. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B. ( 2011 ). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 412 â 422. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Zelazo, P.D., Carlson, S.M., & Kesek, A. ( 2008 ). The development of executive function in childhood. In C.A. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience ( 2nd edn., pp. 553 â 574 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Abutalebi, J., & Green, A.W. ( 2008 ). Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: neural evidence from language switching studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 557 â 582. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J.A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J.A., Castillo, A. et al. ( 2014 ). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 398. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00398 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bedore, L.M., Pena, E.D., Summers, C.L., Boerger, K.M., & Resendiz, M.D. et al. ( 2012 ). The measure matters: language dominance profiles across measures in Spanishâ English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 616 â 629. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. ( 1995 ). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 57, 289 â 300. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bialystok, E. ( 1999 ). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70, 636 â 644. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., & Freedman, M. ( 2007 ). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 459 â 464. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. ( 2004 ). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290 â 303. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., & Luk, G. ( 2008 ). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 859 â 873. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., & Luk, G. ( 2012 ). Bilingualism: consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 240 â 250. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Brigadoi, S., Ceccherini, L., Cutini, S., Scarpa, F., Scatturin, P. et al. ( 2014 ). Motion artifacts in functional nearâ infrared spectroscopy: a comparison of motion correction techniques applied to real cognitive data. NeuroImage, 85, 181 â 191. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Brownell, R. ( 2000 ). Receptive Oneâ Word Picture Vocabulary Test (ROWPVT) Spanishâ Bilingual Edition. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bunge, S.A., Dudukovic, N.M., Thomason, M.E., Vaidya, C.J., & Gabrieli, J.D.E. ( 2002 ). Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: evidence from fMRI. Neuron, 33, 301 â 311. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bunge, S.A., & Zelazo, P.D. ( 2006 ). A brainâ based account of the development of rule use in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 118 â 121. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Carlson, S.M., & Meltzoff, A.N. ( 2008 ). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11, 282 â 298. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastiánâ Gallés, N. ( 2008 ). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 59 â 86. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costaâ Faidella, J., & Sebastiánâ Gallés, N. ( 2009 ). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135 â 149. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Dale, A.M. ( 1999 ). Optimal experimental design for eventâ related fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 8, 109 â 114. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Davidson, M.C., Amso, D., Anderson, L.C., & Diamond, A. ( 2006 ). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13Â years: evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2037 â 2078. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Dean, R.B., & Dixon, W.J. ( 1951 ). Simplified statistics for small numbers of observations. Analytical Chemistry, 23, 636 â 638. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | De Bruin, A., Treccani, B., & Della Sala, S. ( 2015 ). Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: an example of publication bias? Psychological Science, 26, 99 â 107. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. ( 2011 ). The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 254 â 262. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Dong, Y., & Li, P. ( 2015 ). The cognition science of bilingualism. Language and Linguistics Compass, 9, 1 â 13. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Durston, S., Davidson, M.C., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Spicer, J. et al. ( 2006 ). A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Developmental Science, 9, 1 â 20. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Durston, S., Thomas, K., Yang, Y., Ulug, A., Zimmerman, R. et al. ( 2002 ). A neural basis for development of inhibitory control. Developmental Science, 5, 9 â 16. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fan, J., McCandliss, B.D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J.I., & Posner, M.I. ( 2005 ). The activation of attentional networks. NeuroImage, 26, 471 â 479. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fan, J., McCandliss, B.D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M.I. ( 2002 ). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340 â 347. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Francis, D., Carlo, M., August, D., Kenyon, D., Malabonga, V. et al. ( 2001 ). Test of Phonological Processing in Spanish (TOPPS). Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Friston, K.J., Ashburner, J.T., Kiebel, S.J., Nichols, T.E., & Penny, W.D. ( 2006 ). Statistical parametric mapping: The analysis of functional brain images. London: Academic Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Garbin, G., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Bustamante, J.C., Rodriguezâ Pujadas, A. et al. ( 2010 ). Bridging language and attention: brain basis of the impact of bilingualism on cognitive control. NeuroImage, 53, 1272 â 1278. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Green, D.W. ( 2011 ). Language control in different contexts: the behavioral ecology of bilingual speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 103. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00103 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Green, D.W., & Abutalebi, J. ( 2013 ). Language control in bilinguals: the adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 515 â 530. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hernandez, A., Li, P., & MacWhinney, B. ( 2005 ). The emergence of competing modules in bilingualism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 220 â 225. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hilchey, M.D., & Klein, R.M. ( 2011 ). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 18, 625 â 658. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hu, X.S., Hong, K.S., Ge, S.S., & Jeong, M.Y. ( 2010 ). Kalman estimatorâ and general linear modelâ based onâ line brain activation mapping by nearâ infrared spectroscopy. Biomedical Engineering Online, 9. doi: 10.1186/1475â 925Xâ 9â 82 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Huppert, T.J., Diamond, S.G., Franceschini, M.A., & Boas, D.A. ( 2009 ). HomER: a review of timesâ series analysis methods for nearâ infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Applied Optics, 48, D280 â D298. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Johnson, M.H. ( 2001 ). Functional brain development in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 475 â 483. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Johnson, M.H. ( 2011 ). Interactive specialization: a domainâ general framework for human functional brain development? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 7 â 21. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Jurcak, V., Tsuzuki, D., & Dan, I. ( 2007 ). 10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 systems revisited: their validity as relative headâ surfaceâ based positioning systems. NeuroImage, 34, 1600 â 1611. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kapa, L.L., & Colombo, J. ( 2013 ). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive Development, 28, 233 â 246. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, N. ( 2004 ). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test Second Edition (KBITâ 2). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Konrad, K., Neufang, S., Thiel, C.M., Specht, K., Hanisch, C. et al. ( 2005 ). Development of attentional networks: an fMRI study with children and adults. NeuroImage, 28, 429 â 439. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kovács, A.M., & Mehler, J. ( 2009 ). Cognitive gains in 7â monthâ old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 106, 6556 â 6560. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kovelman, I., Baker, S.A., & Petitto, L.A. ( 2008 ). Age of bilingual language exposure as a new window into bilingual reading development. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 11, 203 â 223. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Krizman, J., Marian, V., Shook, A., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. ( 2012 ). Subcortical encoding of sound is enhanced in bilinguals and relates to executive function advantages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 109, 7877 â 7881. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kroll, J.F. ( 2015 ). On the consequences of bilingualism: we need language and the brain to understand cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 32 â 34. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kroll, J.F., & Bialystok, E. ( 2013 ). Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 497 â 514. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kroll, J.F., Dussias, P.E., Bice, K., & Perrotti, L. ( 2015 ). Bilingualism, mind, and brain. Annual Review of Linguistics, 1, 377 â 394. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lloydâ Fox, S., Papademetriou, M., Darboe, M.K., Everdell, N.L., Wegmuller, R. et al. ( 2014 ). Functional nearâ infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to assess cognitive function in infants in rural Africa. Nature Scientific Reports, 4, 4740. doi: 10.1038/srep04740 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Luk, G., Green, D.W., Abutalebi, J., & Grady, C. ( 2012 ). Cognitive control for language switching in bilinguals: a quantitative metaâ analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 1479 â 1488. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Marian, V., & Spivey, M. ( 2003 ). Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Linguistics, 24, 173 â 193. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mazuka, R., Jincho, N., & Oishi, H. ( 2009 ). Development of executive control and language processing. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3, 59 â 89. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mezzacappa, E. ( 2004 ). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75, 1373 â 1386. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Moriguchi, Y., Sakata, Y., Ishibashi, M., & Ishikawa, Y. ( 2015 ). Teaching others ruleâ use improves executive function and prefrontal activations in young children. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 894. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00894 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Morton, J.B., & Harper, S.N. ( 2009 ). Bilinguals show an advantage in cognitive control â the question is why. Developmental Science, 12, 502 â 503. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Muñozâ Sandoval, A.F., Woodcock, R.W., McGrew, K.S., & Mather, N. ( 2005 ). BaterÃa III Woodcockâ Muñoz: Pruebas de Aprovechamiento. Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside Publishing. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Nee, D.E., Wager, T.D., & Jonides, J. ( 2007 ). Interference resolution: insights from a metaâ analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 1 â 17. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Norton, E.S., & Wolf, M. ( 2012 ). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 427 â 452. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Novick, J.M., Trueswell, J.C., & Thompsonâ Schill, S.L. ( 2005 ). Cognitive control and parsing: reexamining the role of Broca’s area in sentence comprehension. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 263 â 281. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Novick, J.M., Trueswell, J.C., & Thompsonâ Schill, S.L. ( 2010 ). Broca’s area and language processing: evidence for the cognitive control connection. Language and Linguistics Compass, 4, 906 â 924. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Park, J., Hebrank, A., Polk, T.A., & Park, D.C. ( 2012 ). Neural dissociation of number from letter recognition and its relationship to parietal numerical processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 39 â 50. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Petitto, L.A., & Kovelman, I. ( 2003 ). The bilingual paradox:Â how signingâ speaking bilingual children help us resolve it and teach us about the brain’s mechanisms underlying all language acquisition. Learning Languages, 8, 5 â 18. | |
dc.owningcollname | Interdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed |
Files in this item
Remediation of Harmful Language
The University of Michigan Library aims to describe its collections in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in them. We encourage you to Contact Us anonymously if you encounter harmful or problematic language in catalog records or finding aids. More information about our policies and practices is available at Remediation of Harmful Language.
Accessibility
If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.