Acoustic sequences in nonâ human animals: a tutorial review and prospectus
dc.contributor.author | Kershenbaum, Arik | |
dc.contributor.author | Blumstein, Daniel T. | |
dc.contributor.author | Roch, Marie A. | |
dc.contributor.author | Akçay, Çağlar | |
dc.contributor.author | Backus, Gregory | |
dc.contributor.author | Bee, Mark A. | |
dc.contributor.author | Bohn, Kirsten | |
dc.contributor.author | Cao, Yan | |
dc.contributor.author | Carter, Gerald | |
dc.contributor.author | Cäsar, Cristiane | |
dc.contributor.author | Coen, Michael | |
dc.contributor.author | DeRuiter, Stacy L. | |
dc.contributor.author | Doyle, Laurance | |
dc.contributor.author | Edelman, Shimon | |
dc.contributor.author | Ferrer‐i‐cancho, Ramon | |
dc.contributor.author | Freeberg, Todd M. | |
dc.contributor.author | Garland, Ellen C. | |
dc.contributor.author | Gustison, Morgan | |
dc.contributor.author | Harley, Heidi E. | |
dc.contributor.author | Huetz, Chloé | |
dc.contributor.author | Hughes, Melissa | |
dc.contributor.author | Hyland Bruno, Julia | |
dc.contributor.author | Ilany, Amiyaal | |
dc.contributor.author | Jin, Dezhe Z. | |
dc.contributor.author | Johnson, Michael | |
dc.contributor.author | Ju, Chenghui | |
dc.contributor.author | Karnowski, Jeremy | |
dc.contributor.author | Lohr, Bernard | |
dc.contributor.author | Manser, Marta B. | |
dc.contributor.author | McCowan, Brenda | |
dc.contributor.author | Mercado, Eduardo | |
dc.contributor.author | Narins, Peter M. | |
dc.contributor.author | Piel, Alex | |
dc.contributor.author | Rice, Megan | |
dc.contributor.author | Salmi, Roberta | |
dc.contributor.author | Sasahara, Kazutoshi | |
dc.contributor.author | Sayigh, Laela | |
dc.contributor.author | Shiu, Yu | |
dc.contributor.author | Taylor, Charles | |
dc.contributor.author | Vallejo, Edgar E. | |
dc.contributor.author | Waller, Sara | |
dc.contributor.author | Zamora‐gutierrez, Veronica | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2017-06-16T20:09:45Z | |
dc.date.available | 2017-06-16T20:09:45Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2016-02 | |
dc.identifier.citation | Kershenbaum, Arik; Blumstein, Daniel T.; Roch, Marie A.; Akçay, Çağlar ; Backus, Gregory; Bee, Mark A.; Bohn, Kirsten; Cao, Yan; Carter, Gerald; Cäsar, Cristiane ; Coen, Michael; DeRuiter, Stacy L.; Doyle, Laurance; Edelman, Shimon; Ferrer‐i‐cancho, Ramon ; Freeberg, Todd M.; Garland, Ellen C.; Gustison, Morgan; Harley, Heidi E.; Huetz, Chloé ; Hughes, Melissa; Hyland Bruno, Julia; Ilany, Amiyaal; Jin, Dezhe Z.; Johnson, Michael; Ju, Chenghui; Karnowski, Jeremy; Lohr, Bernard; Manser, Marta B.; McCowan, Brenda; Mercado, Eduardo; Narins, Peter M.; Piel, Alex; Rice, Megan; Salmi, Roberta; Sasahara, Kazutoshi; Sayigh, Laela; Shiu, Yu; Taylor, Charles; Vallejo, Edgar E.; Waller, Sara; Zamora‐gutierrez, Veronica (2016). "Acoustic sequences in nonâ human animals: a tutorial review and prospectus." Biological Reviews 91(1): 13-52. | |
dc.identifier.issn | 1464-7931 | |
dc.identifier.issn | 1469-185X | |
dc.identifier.uri | https://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/137304 | |
dc.description.abstract | Animal acoustic communication often takes the form of complex sequences, made up of multiple distinct acoustic units. Apart from the wellâ known example of birdsong, other animals such as insects, amphibians, and mammals (including bats, rodents, primates, and cetaceans) also generate complex acoustic sequences. Occasionally, such as with birdsong, the adaptive role of these sequences seems clear (e.g. mate attraction and territorial defence). More often however, researchers have only begun to characterise â  let alone understand â  the significance and meaning of acoustic sequences. Hypotheses abound, but there is little agreement as to how sequences should be defined and analysed. Our review aims to outline suitable methods for testing these hypotheses, and to describe the major limitations to our current and nearâ future knowledge on questions of acoustic sequences. This review and prospectus is the result of a collaborative effort between 43 scientists from the fields of animal behaviour, ecology and evolution, signal processing, machine learning, quantitative linguistics, and information theory, who gathered for a 2013 workshop entitled, â Analysing vocal sequences in animalsâ . Our goal is to present not just a review of the state of the art, but to propose a methodological framework that summarises what we suggest are the best practices for research in this field, across taxa and across disciplines. We also provide a tutorialâ style introduction to some of the most promising algorithmic approaches for analysing sequences. We divide our review into three sections: identifying the distinct units of an acoustic sequence, describing the different ways that information can be contained within a sequence, and analysing the structure of that sequence. Each of these sections is further subdivided to address the key questions and approaches in that area. We propose a uniform, systematic, and comprehensive approach to studying sequences, with the goal of clarifying research terms used in different fields, and facilitating collaboration and comparative studies. Allowing greater interdisciplinary collaboration will facilitate the investigation of many important questions in the evolution of communication and sociality. | |
dc.publisher | Blackwell Publishing Ltd | |
dc.publisher | Wiley Periodicals, Inc. | |
dc.subject.other | Markov model | |
dc.subject.other | meaning | |
dc.subject.other | network analysis | |
dc.subject.other | sequence analysis | |
dc.subject.other | vocalisation | |
dc.subject.other | acoustic communication | |
dc.subject.other | information | |
dc.subject.other | information theory | |
dc.subject.other | machine learning | |
dc.title | Acoustic sequences in nonâ human animals: a tutorial review and prospectus | |
dc.type | Article | en_US |
dc.rights.robots | IndexNoFollow | |
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevel | Ecology and Evolutionary Biology | |
dc.subject.hlbtoplevel | Science | |
dc.description.peerreviewed | Peer Reviewed | |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/137304/1/brv12160.pdf | |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/137304/2/brv12160_am.pdf | |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1111/brv.12160 | |
dc.identifier.source | Biological Reviews | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Parejo, D., Aviles, J. M. & Rodriguez, J. ( 2012 ). Alarm calls modulate the spatial structure of a breeding owl community. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 279, 2135 â 2141. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Scott, J. & Carrington, P. J. ( 2011 ). The SAGE Handbook of Social Network Analysis. SAGE Publications, London. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Searcy, W. A. ( 1992 ). Song repertoire and mate choice in birds. American Zoologist 32, 71 â 80. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Searcy, W. A. & Andersson, M. ( 1986 ). Sexual selection and the evolution of song. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 17, 507 â 533. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Searcy, W. A. & Beecher, M. D. ( 2011 ). Continued scepticism that song overlapping is a signal. Animal Behaviour 81, e1 â e4. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Searcy, W. A. & Nowicki, S. ( 2005 ). The Evolution of Animal Communication: Reliability and Deception in Signaling Systems. Princeton University Press, Princeton. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Searcy, W. A., Nowicki, S. & Peters, S. ( 1999 ). Song types as fundamental units in vocal repertoires. Animal Behaviour 58, 37 â 44. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Searcy, W. A., Podos, J., Peters, S. & Nowicki, S. ( 1995 ). Discrimination of song types and variants in song sparrows. Animal Behaviour 49, 1219 â 1226. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Seddon, N. & Tobias, J. A. ( 2007 ). Song divergence at the edge of Amazonia: an empirical test of the peripatric speciation model. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 90, 173 â 188. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Seyfarth, R. M. & Cheney, D. L. ( 2010 ). Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations. Brain and Language 115, 92 â 100. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. & Bergman, T. J. ( 2005 ). Primate social cognition and the origins of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9, 264 â 266. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., Bergman, T., Fischer, J., Zuberbühler, K. & Hammerschmidt, K. ( 2010 ). The central importance of information in studies of animal communication. Animal Behaviour 80, 3 â 8. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Shannon, P., Markiel, A., Ozier, O., Baliga, N. S., Wang, J. T., Ramage, D., Amin, N., Schwikowski, B. & Ideker, T. ( 2003 ). Cytoscape: a software environment for integrated models of biomolecular interaction networks. Genome Research 13, 2498 â 2504. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Shannon, C. E., Weaver, W., Blahut, R. E. & Hajek, B. ( 1949 ). The Mathematical Theory of Communication. University of Illinois Press, Urbana. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Shapiro, A. D., Tyack, P. L. & Seneff, S. ( 2010 ). Comparing callâ based versus subunitâ based methods for categorizing Norwegian killer whale, Orcinus orca, vocalizations. Animal Behaviour 81, 377 â 386. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Slabbekoorn, H. & Smith, T. B. ( 2002 ). Bird song, ecology and speciation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences 357, 493 â 503. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Slocombe, K. E. & Zuberbühler, K. ( 2006 ). Foodâ associated calls in chimpanzees: responses to food types or food preferences? Animal Behaviour 72, 989 â 999. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Smith, W. J. ( 1977 ). The Behavior of Communicating. Harvard University Press, Cambridge. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Smith, J. N., Goldizen, A. W., Dunlop, R. A. & Noad, M. J. ( 2008 ). Songs of male humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae, are involved in intersexual interactions. Animal Behaviour 76, 467 â 477. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Snijders, T. A. ( 2002 ). Markov chain Monte Carlo estimation of exponential random graph models. Journal of Social Structure 3, 1 â 40. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Solan, Z., Horn, D., Ruppin, E. & Edelman, S. ( 2005 ). Unsupervised learning of natural languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102, 11629 â 11634. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Specht, R. ( 2004 ). Avisoftâ SASLab Pro. Avisoft, Berlin. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Stegmann, U. ( 2013 ). Animal Communication Theory: Information and Influence. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Stoddard, P. ( 1996 ). Vocal recognition of neighbors by territorial passerines. In Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds (eds D. E. Kroodsma and E. L. Miller ), pp. 356 â 374. Cornell University Press, Ithaca. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Stolcke, A. ( 2002 ). SRILMâ an extensible language modeling toolkit. In International Conference on Spoken Language Processing, Volume 2, pp. 901 â 904. Denver. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Sturdy, C. B., Phillmore, L. S. & Weisman, R. G. ( 2000 ). Callâ note discriminations in blackâ capped chickadees ( Poecile atricapillus ). Journal of Comparative Psychology 114, 357. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Suthers, R. A. ( 1997 ). Peripheral control and lateralization of birdsong. Journal of Neurobiology 33, 632 â 652. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Suthers, R. A. ( 2004 ). How birds sing and why it matters. In Nature’s Music: The Science of Birdsong (eds P. R. Marler and H. Slabbekoorn ), pp. 272 â 295. Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Taylor, A. M., Reby, D. & Mccomb, K. ( 2008 ). Human listeners attend to size information in domestic dog growls. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 123, 2903 â 2909. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Tchernichovski, O., Nottebohm, F., Ho, C. E., Pesaran, B. & Mitra, P. P. ( 2000 ). A procedure for an automated measurement of song similarity. Animal Behaviour 59, 1167 â 1176. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ten Cate, C., Lachlan, R. & Zuidema, W. ( 2013 ). Analyzing the structure of bird vocalizations and language: finding common ground. In Birdsong, Speech, and Language: Exploring the Evolution of Mind and Brain (eds J. J. Bolhuis and M. Everaert ), pp. 243 â 260. MIT Press, Cambridge. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Thomas, J. A., Zinnel, K. C. & Ferm, L. M. ( 1983 ). Analysis of Weddell seal ( Leptonychotes weddelli ) vocalizations using underwater playbacks. Canadian Journal of Zoology 61, 1448 â 1456. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Thomson, D. J. ( 1982 ). Spectrum estimation and harmonic analysis. Proceedings of the IEEE 70, 1055 â 1096. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Thompson, A. B. & Hare, J. F. ( 2010 ). Neighbourhood watch: multiple alarm callers communicate directional predator movement in Richardson’s ground squirrels, Spermophilus richardsonii. Animal Behaviour 80, 269 â 275. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Titze, I. R. ( 1994 ). Principles of Voice Production. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Todt, D. & Hultsch, H. ( 1998 ). How songbirds deal with large amounts of serial information: retrieval rules suggest a hierarchical song memory. Biological Cybernetics 79, 487 â 500. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Toews, D. P. & Irwin, D. E. ( 2008 ). Cryptic speciation in a Holarctic passerine revealed by genetic and bioacoustic analyses. Molecular Ecology 17, 2691 â 2705. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Trawicki, M. B., Johnson, M. & Osiejuk, T. ( 2005 ). Automatic songâ type classification and speaker identification of Norwegian Ortolan Bunting ( Emberiza hortulana ) vocalizations. In IEEE Workshop on Machine Learning for Signal Processing, pp. 277 â 282. Mystic. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Tu, H. & Dooling, R. J. ( 2012 ). Perception of warble song in budgerigars ( Melopsittacus undulatus ): evidence for special processing. Animal Cognition 15, 1151 â 1159. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Tu, H., Smith, E. W. & Dooling, R. J. ( 2011 ). Acoustic and perceptual categories of vocal elements in the warble song of budgerigars ( Melopsittacus undulatus ). Journal of Comparative Psychology 125, 420 â 430. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Tyack, P. ( 1983 ). Differential response of humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae, to playback of song or social sounds. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 13, 49 â 55. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Van Heijningen, C. A., De Visser, J., Zuidema, W. & Ten Cate, C. ( 2009 ). Simple rules can explain discrimination of putative recursive syntactic structures by a songbird species. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106, 20538 â 20543. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Van Schaik, C. P., Damerius, L. & Isler, K. ( 2013 ). Wild orangutan males plan and communicate their travel direction one day in advance. PLoS ONE 8, e74896. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Vehrencamp, S. L., Hall, M. L., Bohman, E. R., Depeine, C. D. & Dalziell, A. H. ( 2007 ). Song matching, overlapping, and switching in the banded wren: the sender’s perspective. Behavioral Ecology 18, 849 â 859. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Waller, S. ( 2012 ). Science of the monkey mind: primate penchants and human pursuits. In Experiencing Animal Minds: An Anthology of Animalâ human Encounters (eds J. A. Smith and R. W. Mitchell ), pp. 79 â 94. Columbia University Press, New York. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Watkins, W. A. & Schevill, W. E. ( 1977 ). Sperm whale codas. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 62, 1485 â 1490. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Watts, D. J. & Strogatz, S. H. ( 1998 ). Collective dynamics of â smallâ worldâ networks. Nature 393, 440 â 442. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Weary, D., Krebs, J., Eddyshaw, R., Mcgregor, P. & Horn, A. ( 1988 ). Decline in song output by great tits: exhaustion or motivation? Animal Behaviour 36, 1242 â 1244. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Weary, D. M., Lambrechts, M. & Krebs, J. ( 1991 ). Does singing exhaust male great tits? Animal Behaviour 41, 540 â 542. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Weary, D. M. & Lemon, R. E. ( 1988 ). Evidence against the continuityâ versatility relationship in bird song. Animal Behaviour 36, 1379 â 1383. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Weary, D. M. & Lemon, R. E. ( 1990 ). Kroodsma refuted. Animal Behaviour 39, 996 â 998. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Weiss, R. J. & Bello, J. P. ( 2011 ). Unsupervised discovery of temporal structure in music. IEEE Journal on Selected Topics in Signal Processing 5, 1240 â 1251. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Weiss, D. J. & Hauser, M. D. ( 2002 ). Perception of harmonics in the combination long call of cottontop tamarins, Saguinus oedipus. Animal Behaviour 64, 415 â 426. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Weiss, M., Hultsch, H., Adam, I., Scharff, C. & Kipper, S. ( 2014 ). The use of network analysis to study complex animal communication systems: a study on nightingale song. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 281, 20140460. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wernicke, S. & Rasche, F. ( 2006 ). FANMOD: a tool for fast network motif detection. Bioinformatics 22, 1152 â 1153. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wheeler, B. C. ( 2010 a ). Decrease in alarm call response among tufted capuchins in competitive feeding contexts: possible evidence for counterdeception. International Journal of Primatology 31, 665 â 675. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wheeler, B. C. ( 2010 b ). Production and perception of situationally variable alarm calls in wild tufted capuchin monkeys ( Cebus apella nigritus ). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 64, 989 â 1000. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wheeler, B. C. & Hammerschmidt, K. ( 2012 ). Proximate factors underpinning receiver responses to deceptive false alarm calls in wild tufted capuchin monkeys: is it counterdeception? American Journal of Primatology 75, 715 â 725. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wiens, J. A. ( 1982 ). Song pattern variation in the sage sparrow ( Amphispiza belli ): dialects or epiphenomena? The Auk 99, 208 â 229. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wiley, R. H. ( 1983 ). The evolution of communication: information and manipulation. Animal Behaviour 2, 156 â 189. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wolpert, D. H. & Macready, W. G. ( 1997 ). No free lunch theorems for optimization. IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation 1, 67 â 82. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wyttenbach, R. A., May, M. L. & Hoy, R. R. ( 1996 ). Categorical perception of sound frequency by crickets. Science 273, 1542 â 1544. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Young, S. J. & Young, S. ( 1994 ). The htk hidden Markov model toolkit: Design and philosophy. Entropic Cambridge Research Laboratory, Ltd 2, 2 â 44. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Zhong, S. & Ghosh, J. ( 2005 ). Generative modelâ based document clustering: a comparative study. Knowledge and Information Systems 8, 374 â 384. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Zipf, G. K. ( 1949 ). Human Behavior and the Principle of Least Effort. Addisonâ Wesley Press, Oxford. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Zucchini, W. & Macdonald, I. L. ( 2009 ). Hidden Markov Models for Time Series: An Introduction Using R. Chapman & Hall/CRC Press, London. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Adam, O., Cazau, D., Gandilhon, N., Fabre, B., Laitman, J. T. & Reidenberg, J. S. ( 2013 ). New acoustic model for humpback whale sound production. Applied Acoustics 74, 1182 â 1190. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Adami, C. ( 2002 ). What is complexity? BioEssays 24, 1085 â 1094. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Adams, D. C. & Anthony, C. D. ( 1996 ). Using randomization techniques to analyse behavioural data. Animal Behaviour 51, 733 â 738. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Akçay, à ., Tom, M. E., Campbell, S. E. & Beecher, M. D. ( 2013 ). Song type matching is an honest early threat signal in a hierarchical animal communication system. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 280, 20122517. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Anderson, T. W. & Goodman, L. A. ( 1957 ). Statistical inference about Markov chains. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics 28, 89 â 110. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Arayaâ Salas, M. ( 2012 ). Is birdsong music? Significance 9, 4 â 7. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Arcadi, A. C. ( 1996 ). Phrase structure of wild chimpanzee pant hoots: patterns of production and interpopulation variability. American Journal of Primatology 39, 159 â 178. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Arlot, S. & Celisse, A. ( 2010 ). A survey of crossâ validation procedures for model selection. Statistics Surveys 4, 40 â 79. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Arnold, K. & Zuberbühler, K. ( 2006 a ). Language evolution: semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature 441, 303. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Arnold, K. & Zuberbühler, K. ( 2006 b ). The alarmâ calling system of adult male puttyâ nosed monkeys, Cercopithecus nictitans martini. Animal Behaviour 72, 643 â 653. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Arnold, K. & Zuberbühler, K. ( 2008 ). Meaningful call combinations in a nonâ human primate. Current Biology 18, R202 â R203. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Arnold, K. & Zuberbühler, K. ( 2013 ). Female puttyâ nosed monkeys use experimentally altered contextual information to disambiguate the cause of male alarm calls. PLoS ONE 8, e65660. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Arriaga, J. G., Kossan, G., Cody, M. L., Vallejo, E. E. & Taylor, C. E. ( 2013 ). Acoustic sensor arrays for understanding bird communication. Identifying Cassin’s Vireos using SVMs and HMMs. In ECAL 2013, Volume 12, pp. 827 â 828. Taormina. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Attard, M. R., Pitcher, B. J., Charrier, I., Ahonen, H. & Harcourt, R. G. ( 2010 ). Vocal discrimination in mate guarding male Australian sea lions: familiarity breeds contempt. Ethology 116, 704 â 712. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Aubin, T., Jouventin, P. & Hildebrand, C. ( 2000 ). Penguins use the twoâ voice system to recognize each other. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 267, 1081 â 1087. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Aubin, T., Mathevon, N., Staszewski, V. & Boulinier, T. ( 2007 ). Acoustic communication in the Kittiwake Rissa tridactyla: potential cues for sexual and individual signatures in long calls. Polar Biology 30, 1027 â 1033. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Baker, M. C. & Becker, A. M. ( 2002 ). Mobbing calls of blackâ capped chickadees: effects of urgency on call production. The Wilson Bulletin 114, 510 â 516. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Baker, M. C. & Logue, D. M. ( 2003 ). Population differentiation in a complex bird sound: a comparison of three bioacoustical analysis procedures. Ethology 109, 223 â 242. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Baptista, L. F. & Keister, R. A. ( 2005 ). Why birdsong is sometimes like music. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 48, 426 â 443. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Baronchelli, A., Ferrerâ Iâ Cancho, R., Pastorâ Satorras, R., Chater, N. & Christiansen, M. H. ( 2013 ). Networks in cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17, 348 â 360. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Barrat, A., Barthelemy, M., Pastorâ Satorras, R. & Vespignani, A. ( 2004 ). The architecture of complex weighted networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101, 3747 â 3752. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bastian, M., Heymann, S. & Jacomy, M. ( 2009 ). Gephi: an open source software for exploring and manipulating networks. In Proceedings of the Third International ICWSM Conference, San Jose. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Baugh, A., Akre, K. & Ryan, M. ( 2008 ). Categorical perception of a natural, multivariate signal: mating call recognition in túngara frogs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105, 8985 â 8988. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bazúaâ Durán, C. & Au, W. W. ( 2002 ). The whistles of Hawaiian spinner dolphins. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 112, 3064 â 3072. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Beckers, G. J., Bolhuis, J. J., Okanoya, K. & Berwick, R. C. ( 2012 ). Birdsong neurolinguistics: songbird contextâ free grammar claim is premature. Neuroreport 23, 139 â 145. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bee, M. A. & Micheyl, C. ( 2008 ). The cocktail party problem: what is it? How can it be solved? And why should animal behaviorists study it? Journal of Comparative Psychology 122, 235 â 251. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Beecher, M. D., Campbell, S. E., Burt, J. M., Hill, C. E. & Nordby, J. C. ( 2000 ). Songâ type matching between neighbouring song sparrows. Animal Behaviour 59, 21 â 27. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Benâ David, S., Von Luxburg, U. & Pál, D. ( 2006 ). A sober look at clustering stability. In Learning Theory, pp. 5 â 19. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bennetâ Clark, H. & Ewing, A. ( 1969 ). Pulse interval as a critical parameter in the courtship song of Drosophila melanogaster. Animal Behaviour 17, 755 â 759. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bergman, T. J., Beehner, J. C., Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M. ( 2003 ). Hierarchical classification by rank and kinship in baboons. Science 302, 1234 â 1236. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Berwick, R. C., Okanoya, K., Beckers, G. J. L. & Bolhuis, J. J. ( 2011 ). Songs to syntax: the linguistics of birdsong. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15, 113 â 121. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bizley, J. K. & Cohen, Y. E. ( 2013 ). The what, where and how of auditoryâ object perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14, 693 â 707. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Blumstein, D. T. ( 1995 ). Goldenâ marmot alarm calls. II. Asymmetrical production and perception of situationally specific vocalizations? Ethology 101, 25 â 32. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Blumstein, D. T. ( 2007 ). The evolution, function, and meaning of marmot alarm communication. Advances in the Study of Behavior 37, 371 â 401. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Blumstein, D. T. & Munos, O. ( 2005 ). Individual, age and sexâ specific information is contained in yellowâ bellied marmot alarm calls. Animal Behaviour 69, 353 â 361. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Blumstein, D. T., Verneyre, L. & Daniel, J. C. ( 2004 ). Reliability and the adaptive utility of discrimination among alarm callers. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 271, 1851 â 1857. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bohm, D. ( 1989 ). Meaning and information. In The Search for Meaning, The New Spirit in Science and Philosophy (ed. P. Pylkkänen ), pp. 43 â 62. Thorsons Publishing Group, Wellingborough. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bohn, K. M., Schmidtâ French, B., Ma, S. T. & Pollak, G. D. ( 2008 ). Syllable acoustics, temporal patterns, and call composition vary with behavioral context in Mexican freeâ tailed bats. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 124, 1838 â 1848. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bohn, K. M., Schmidtâ French, B., Schwartz, C., Smotherman, M. & Pollak, G. D. ( 2009 ). Versatility and stereotypy of freeâ tailed bat songs. PLoS ONE 4, e6746. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bonner, J. T. ( 1988 ). The Evolution of Complexity by Means of Natural Selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Borgatti, S. P., Everett, M. G. & Freeman, L. C. ( 2002 ). Ucinet for Windows: Software for Social Network Analysis. Analytic Technologies, Harvard. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bradbury, J. W. & Vehrencamp, S. L. ( 2011 ). Principles of Animal Communication. Sinauer, Sunderland. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Briefer, E., Aubin, T., Lehongre, K. & Rybak, F. ( 2008 ). How to identify dear enemies: the group signature in the complex song of the skylark Alauda arvensis. Journal of Experimental Biology 211, 317 â 326. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Briefer, E., Osiejuk, T. S., Rybak, F. & Aubin, T. ( 2010 ). Are bird song complexity and song sharing shaped by habitat structure? An information theory and statistical approach. Journal of Theoretical Biology 262, 151 â 164. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Briefer, E. F., Rybak, F. & Aubin, T. ( 2013 ). Does true syntax or simple auditory object support the role of skylark song dialect? Animal Behaviour 86, 1131 â 1137. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Brown, S. D., Dooling, R. J. & O’Grady, K. E. ( 1988 ). Perceptual organization of acoustic stimuli by budgerigars ( Melopsittacus undulatus ): III. Contact calls. Journal of Comparative Psychology 102, 236 â 247. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Brumm, H. & Slater, P. J. ( 2006 ). Ambient noise, motor fatigue, and serial redundancy in chaffinch song. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 60, 475 â 481. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Buck, J. R. & Tyack, P. L. ( 1993 ). A quantitative measure of similarity for Tursiops truncatus signature whistles. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 94, 2497 â 2506. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Burghardt, G. M., Bartmessâ Levasseur, J. N., Browning, S. A., Morrison, K. E., Stec, C. L., Zachau, C. E. & Freeberg, T. M. ( 2012 ). Perspectivesâ minimizing observer bias in behavioral studies: a review and recommendations. Ethology 118, 511 â 517. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Burt, J. M., Campbell, S. E. & Beecher, M. D. ( 2001 ). Song type matching as threat: a test using interactive playback. Animal Behaviour 62, 1163 â 1170. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Byers, B. E. & Kroodsma, D. E. ( 2009 ). Female mate choice and songbird song repertoires. Animal Behaviour 77, 13 â 22. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Caldwell, M. C. ( 1965 ). Individualized whistle contours in bottleâ nosed dolphins ( Tursiops truncatus ). Nature 207, 434 â 435. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Caldwell, M. C., Caldwell, D. K. & Tyack, P. L. ( 1990 ). Review of the signatureâ whistle hypothesis for the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin. In The Bottlenose Dolphin (eds S. Leatherwood and R. R. Reeves ), pp. 199 â 234. Academic Press, San Diego. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Call, J. E. & Tomasello, M. E. ( 2007 ). The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys. Taylor & Francis Group/Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New York. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cane, V. R. ( 1959 ). Behaviour sequences as semiâ Markov chains. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B: Methodological 21, 36 â 58. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cappé, O., Moulines, E. & Rydén, T. ( 2005 ). Inference in Hidden Markov Models. Springer Science Business Media, New York. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Carter, G. G., Skowronski, M. D., Faure, P. A. & Fenton, B. ( 2008 ). Antiphonal calling allows individual discrimination in whiteâ winged vampire bats. Animal Behaviour 76, 1343 â 1355. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cäsar, C., Byrne, R. W., Hoppitt, W., Young, R. J. & Zuberbühler, K. ( 2012a ). Evidence for semantic communication in titi monkey alarm calls. Animal Behaviour 84, 405 â 411. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cäsar, C., Byrne, R., Young, R. J. & Zuberbühler, K. ( 2012 b ). The alarm call system of wild blackâ fronted titi monkeys, Callicebus nigrifrons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 66, 653 â 667. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cäsar, C., Zuberbühler, K., Young, R. J. & Byrne, R. W. ( 2013 ). Titi monkey call sequences vary with predator location and type. Biology Letters 9, 20130535. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Catchpole, C. K. ( 1976 ). Temporal and sequential organisation of song in the sedge warbler ( Acrocephalus schoenobaenus ). Behaviour 59, 226 â 246. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Catchpole, C. K. & Slater, P. J. B. ( 2003 ). Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cazau, D., Adam, O., Laitman, J. T. & Reidenberg, J. S. ( 2013 ). Understanding the intentional acoustic behavior of humpback whales: a productionâ based approach. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 134, 2268 â 2273. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ceugniet, M. & Aubin, T. ( 2001 ). The rally call recognition in males of two hybridizing partridge species, redâ legged ( Alectoris rufa ) and rock ( A. graeca ) partridges. Behavioural Processes 55, 1 â 12. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Charif, R., Ponirakis, D. & Krein, T. ( 2006 ). Raven Lite 1.0 User’s Guide. Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Charrier, I., Ahonen, H. & Harcourt, R. G. ( 2011 ). What makes an Australian sea lion ( Neophoca cinerea ) male’s bark threatening? Journal of Comparative Psychology 125, 385. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Charrier, I. & Harcourt, R. G. ( 2006 ). Individual vocal identity in mother and pup Australian sea lions ( Neophoca cinerea ). Journal of Mammalogy 87, 929 â 938. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Charrier, I., Lee, T. T., Bloomfield, L. L. & Sturdy, C. B. ( 2005 ). Acoustic mechanisms of noteâ type perception in blackâ capped chickadee ( Poecile atricapillus ) calls. Journal of Comparative Psychology 119, 371. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Charrier, I., Mathevon, N. & Aubin, T. ( 2013 ). Bearded seal males perceive geographic variation in their trills. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 67, 1679 â 1689. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Charrier, I., Mathevon, N. & Jouventin, P. ( 2003 ). Individuality in the voice of fur seal females: an analysis study of the pup attraction call in Arctocephalus tropicalis. Marine Mammal Science 19, 161 â 172. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Charrier, I., Pitcher, B. J. & Harcourt, R. G. ( 2009 ). Vocal recognition of mothers by Australian sea lion pups: individual signature and environmental constraints. Animal Behaviour 78, 1127 â 1134. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Chatfield, C. & Lemon, R. E. ( 1970 ). Analysing sequences of behavioural events. Journal of Theoretical Biology 29, 427 â 445. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Chatterjee, A. ( 2005 ). A madness to the methods in cognitive neuroscience? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17, 847 â 849. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Chen, C. & Fushing, H. ( 2012 ). Multiscale community geometry in a network and its application. Physical Review E 86, 041120. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cholewiak, D. M., Sousaâ Lima, R. S. & Cerchio, S. ( 2012 ). Humpback whale song hierarchical structure: historical context and discussion of current classification issues. Marine Mammal Science 29, E312 â E332. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Chomsky, N. ( 1957 ). Syntactic Structures. de Gruyter Mouton, The Hague. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Christiansen, M. H. & Chater, N. ( 2001 ). Connectionist psycholinguistics: capturing the empirical data. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5, 82 â 88. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Clark, A. ( 2010 ). Towards general algorithms for grammatical inference. Lecture Notes in Computer Science 6331, 11 â 30. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Clark, A., Eyraud, R. & Habrard, A. ( 2010 ). Using contextual representations to efficiently learn contextâ free languages. The Journal of Machine Learning Research 11, 2707 â 2744. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Clark, C. J. & Feo, T. J. ( 2008 ). The Anna’s hummingbird chirps with its tail: a new mechanism of sonation in birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 275, 955 â 962. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Clark, C. J. & Feo, T. J. ( 2010 ). Why do Calypte hummingbirds â singâ with both their tail and their syrinx? An apparent example of sexual sensory bias. The American Naturalist 175, 27 â 37. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Clark, C. W., Marler, P. & Beeman, K. ( 1987 ). Quantitative analysis of animal vocal phonology: an application to swamp sparrow song. Ethology 76, 101 â 115. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Clemins, P. J. & Johnson, M. T. ( 2006 ). Generalized perceptual linear prediction features for animal vocalization analysis. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 120, 527 â 534. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cohn, L. ( 1995 ). Timeâ Frequency Analysis: Theory and Applications, 1995. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Collias, N. E. ( 1987 ). The vocal repertoire of the red junglefowl: a spectrographic classification and the code of communication. Condor 89, 510 â 524. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cover, T. M. & Thomas, J. A. ( 1991 ). Elements of Information Theory. John Wiley & Sons, Inc, New York. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Crockford, C. & Boesch, C. ( 2005 ). Call combinations in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 142, 397 â 421. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Croft, D. P., James, R. & Krause, J. ( 2008 ). Exploring Animal Social Networks. Princeton University Press, Princeton. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Csardi, G. & Nepusz, T. ( 2006 ). The igraph software package for complex network research. International Journal of Complex Systems 1695, 1 â 9. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Curé, C., Aubin, T. & Mathevon, N. ( 2011 ). Sex discrimination and mate recognition by voice in the Yelkouan shearwater Puffinus yelkouan. Bioacoustics 20, 235 â 249. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Curé, C., Mathevon, N., Mundry, R. & Aubin, T. ( 2012 ). Acoustic cues used for species recognition can differ between sexes and sibling species: evidence in shearwaters. Animal Behaviour 84, 239 â 250. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cynx, J. ( 1990 ). Experimental determination of a unit of song production in the zebra finch ( Taeniopygia guttata ). Journal of Comparative Psychology 104, 3 â 10. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cynx, J., Williams, H. & Nottebohm, F. ( 1990 ). Timbre discrimination in Zebra finch ( Taeniopygia guttata ) song syllables. Journal of Comparative Psychology 104, 303 â 308. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Darling, J. D., Jones, M. E. & Nicklin, C. P. ( 2006 ). Humpback whale songs: do they organize males during the breeding season? Behaviour 143, 1051 â 1102. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Dawkins, R. & Krebs, J. R. ( 1978 ). Animal signals: information or manipulation. Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach 2, 282 â 309. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Deecke, V. B. & Janik, V. M. ( 2006 ). Automated categorization of bioacoustic signals: avoiding perceptual pitfalls. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119, 645 â 653. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Denker, M. & WoyczyÅ ski, W. A. ( 1998 ). Introductory Statistics and Random Phenomena: Uncertainty, Complexity and Chaotic Behaviour in Engineering and Science. Springer, Boston. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Dent, M. L., Brittanâ Powell, E. F., Dooling, R. J. & Pierce, A. ( 1997 ). Perception of synthetic/ba/â /wa/speech continuum by budgerigars ( Melopsittacus undulatus ). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 102, 1891 â 1897. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Dentressangle, F., Aubin, T. & Mathevon, N. ( 2012 ). Males use time whereas females prefer harmony: individual call recognition in the dimorphic blueâ footed booby. Animal Behaviour 84, 413 â 420. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Derégnaucourt, S., Mitra, P. P., Fehér, O., Pytte, C. & Tchernichovski, O. ( 2005 ). How sleep affects the developmental learning of bird song. Nature 433, 710 â 716. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Deslandes, V., Faria, L. R., Borges, M. E. & Pie, M. R. ( 2014 ). The structure of an avian syllable syntax network. Behavioural Processes 106, 53 â 59. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Dooling, R. J., Park, T. J., Brown, S. D., Okanoya, K. & Soli, S. D. ( 1987 ). Perceptual organization of acoustic stimuli by budgerigars ( Melopsittacus undulatus ): II. Vocal signals. Journal of Comparative Psychology 101, 367 â 381. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gale, W. & Sampson, G. ( 1995 ). Goodâ Turing smoothing without tears. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics 2, 217 â 237. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Doupe, A. J. & Konishi, M. ( 1991 ). Songâ selective auditory circuits in the vocal control system of the zebra finch. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 88, 11339 â 11343. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Duda, R. O., Hart, P. E. & Stork, D. G. ( 2012 ). Pattern Classification. John Wiley & Sons, New York. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Dunlop, R. A., Noad, M. J., Cato, D. H. & Stokes, D. ( 2007 ). The social vocalization repertoire of east Australian migrating humpback whales ( Megaptera novaeangliae ). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 122, 2893 â 2905. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Eens, M. ( 1997 ). Understanding the complex song of the European starling: an integrated ethological approach. Advances in the Study of Behavior 26, 355 â 434. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Eens, M., Pinxten, R. & Verheyen, R. F. ( 1989 ). Temporal and sequential organization of song bouts in the starling. Ardea 77, 75 â 86. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Eens, M., Pinxten, R. & Verheyen, R. F. ( 1991 ). Male song as a cue for mate choice in the European starling. Behaviour 116, 210 â 238. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Eens, M., Pinxten, R. & Verheyen, R. F. ( 1993 ). Function of the song and song repertoire in the European starling ( Sturnus vulgaris ): an aviary experiment. Behaviour 125, 51 â 66. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Esser, K., Condon, C. J., Suga, N. & Kanwal, J. S. ( 1997 ). Syntax processing by auditory cortical neurons in the FMâ FM area of the mustached bat Pteronotus parnellii. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 94, 14019 â 14024. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Evans, C. S. ( 1997 ). Referential signals. Perspectives in Ethology 12, 99 â 143. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Evans, C. S., Evans, L. & Marler, P. ( 1993 ). On the meaning of alarm calls: functional reference in an avian vocal system. Animal Behaviour 46, 23 â 38. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fagiolo, G. ( 2007 ). Clustering in complex directed networks. Physical Review E 76, 026107. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Falls, J. B. ( 1982 ). Individual recognition by sounds in birds. In Acoustic Communication in Birds (Volume 2, eds D. H. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller ), pp. 237 â 278. Academic Press, New York. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Farabaugh, S. & Dooling, R. ( 1996 ). Acoustic communication in parrots: laboratory and field studies of budgerigars, Melopsittacus undulatus. In Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds, pp. 97 â 117. Cornell University Press, Ithaca. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ferrerâ Iâ Cancho, R. ( 2005 ). Zipf’s law from a communicative phase transition. The European Physical Journal Bâ Condensed Matter and Complex Systems 47, 449 â 457. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ferrerâ Iâ Cancho, R., Hernándezâ Fernández, A., Lusseau, D., Agoramoorthy, G., Hsu, M. J. & Semple, S. ( 2013 ). Compression as a universal principle of animal behavior. Cognitive Science 37, 1565 â 1578. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ferrerâ Iâ Cancho, R. & Mccowan, B. ( 2012 ). The span of correlations in dolphin whistle sequences. Journal of Statistical Mechanics: Theory and Experiment 2012, P06002. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fewell, J. H. ( 2003 ). Social insect networks. Science 301, 1867 â 1870. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fischer, J., Noser, R. & Hammerschmidt, K. ( 2013 ). Bioacoustic field research: a primer to acoustic analyses and playback experiments with primates. American Journal of Primatology 75, 643 â 663. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fitch, W. ( 2000 ). The evolution of speech: a comparative review. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4, 258 â 267. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fitch, W. T. ( 1997 ). Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in rhesus macaques. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 102, 1213 â 1222. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ford, J. K. ( 1989 ). Acoustic behaviour of resident killer whales ( Orcinus orca ) off Vancouver Island, British Columbia. Canadian Journal of Zoology 67, 727 â 745. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fortunato, S. ( 2010 ). Community detection in graphs. Physics Reports 486, 75 â 174. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fred, A. L. & Jain, A. K. ( 2005 ). Combining multiple clusterings using evidence accumulation. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence 27, 835 â 850. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Freeberg, T. M. ( 2012 ). Geographic variation in note composition and use of chickâ aâ dee calls of Carolina chickadees ( Poecile carolinensis ). Ethology 118, 555 â 565. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Freeberg, T. M., Dunbar, R. I. M. & Ord, T. J. ( 2012 ). Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences 367, 1785 â 1801. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Freeberg, T. M. & Lucas, J. R. ( 2012 ). Information theoretical approaches to chickâ aâ dee calls of Carolina chickadees ( Poecile carolinensis ). Journal of Comparative Psychology 126, 68 â 81. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Frumhoff, P. ( 1983 ). Aberrant songs of humpback whales ( Megaptera novaeangliae ): clues to the structure of humpback songs. In Communication and Behavior of Whales (ed. R. Payne ), pp. 81 â 127. Westview Press, Boulder. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fushing, H. & Mcassey, M. P. ( 2010 ). Time, temperature, and data cloud geometry. Physical Review E 82, 061110. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gammon, D. E. & Altizer, C. E. ( 2011 ). Northern Mockingbirds produce syntactical patterns of vocal mimicry that reflect taxonomy of imitated species. Journal of Field Ornithology 82, 158 â 164. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gardner, T. J., Naef, F. & Nottebohm, F. ( 2005 ). Freedom and rules: the acquisition and reprogramming of a bird’s learned song. Science 308, 1046 â 1049. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Garland, E. C., Goldizen, A. W., Rekdahl, M. L., Constantine, R., Garrigue, C., Hauser, N. D., Poole, M. M., Robbins, J. & Noad, M. J. ( 2011 ). Dynamic horizontal cultural transmission of humpback whale song at the ocean basin scale. Current Biology 21, 687 â 691. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Garland, E. C., Lilley, M. S., Goldizen, A. W., Rekdahl, M. L., Garrigue, C. & Noad, M. J. ( 2012 ). Improved versions of the Levenshtein distance method for comparing sequence information in animals’ vocalisations: tests using humpback whale song. Behaviour 149, 1413 â 1441. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Garland, E. C., Noad, M. J., Goldizen, A. W., Lilley, M. S., Rekdahl, M. L., Garrigue, C., Constantine, R., Hauser, N. D., Poole, M. M. & Robbins, J. ( 2013 ). Quantifying humpback whale song sequences to understand the dynamics of song exchange at the ocean basin scale. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 133, 560 â 569. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gentner, T. Q., Fenn, K. M., Margoliash, D. & Nusbaum, H. C. ( 2006 ). Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. Nature 440, 1204 â 1207. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gentner, T. Q. & Hulse, S. H. ( 1998 ). Perceptual mechanisms for individual vocal recognition in European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris. Animal Behaviour 56, 579 â 594. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gerhardt, H. C. ( 2001 ). Acoustic communication in two groups of closely related treefrogs. Advances in the Study of Behavior 30, 99 â 167. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gerhardt, H. C. & Huber, F. ( 2002 ). Acoustic Communication in Insects and Anurans: Common Problems and Diverse Solutions. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ghazanfar, A. A., Flombaum, J. I., Miller, C. T. & Hauser, M. D. ( 2001 ). The units of perception in the antiphonal calling behavior of cottonâ top tamarins ( Saguinus oedipus ): playback experiments with long calls. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 187, 27 â 35. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Girgenrath, M. & Marsh, R. ( 1997 ). In vivo performance of trunk muscles in tree frogs during calling. Journal of Experimental Biology 200, 3101 â 3108. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gold, E. M. ( 1967 ). Language identification in the limit. Information and Control 10, 447 â 474. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Goldbogen, J. A., Southall, B. L., Deruiter, S. L., Calambokidis, J., Friedlaender, A. S., Hazen, E. L., Falcone, E. A., Schorr, G. S., Douglas, A. & Moretti, D. J. ( 2013 ). Blue whales respond to simulated midâ frequency military sonar. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 280, 20130657. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gouzoules, S., Gouzoules, H. & Marler, P. ( 1984 ). Rhesus monkey ( Macaca mulatta ) screams: representational signalling in the recruitment of agonistic aid. Animal Behaviour 32, 182 â 193. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Green, S. R., Mercado Iii, E., Pack, A. A. & Herman, L. M. ( 2011 ). Recurring patterns in the songs of humpback whales ( Megaptera novaeangliae ). Behavioural Processes 86, 284 â 294. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Grieves, L., Logue, D. & Quinn, J. ( 2014 ). Jointâ nesting smoothâ billed anis, Crotophaga ani, use a functionally referential alarm call system. Animal Behaviour 89, 215 â 221. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Griffiths, T. D. & Warren, J. D. ( 2004 ). What is an auditory object? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5, 887 â 892. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gwilliam, J., Charrier, I. & Harcourt, R. G. ( 2008 ). Vocal identity and species recognition in male Australian sea lions, Neophoca cinerea. The Journal of Experimental Biology 211, 2288 â 2295. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hahnloser, R. H., Kozhevnikov, A. A. & Fee, M. S. ( 2002 ). An ultraâ sparse code underlies the generation of neural sequences in a songbird. Nature 419, 65 â 70. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hailman, J. P. ( 2008 ). Coding and Redundancy: Manâ Made and Animalâ Evolved Signals. Harvard University Press, Cambridge. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hamaker, J., Ganapathiraju, A. & Picone, J. ( 1998 ). Information theoretic approaches to model selection. Proceedings of the International Conference on Spoken Language Processing, Volume 7, pp. 2931 â 2934, Sydney. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Handcock, M. S., Hunter, D. R., Butts, C. T., Goodreau, S. M. & Morris, M. ( 2008 ). statnet: software tools for the representation, visualization, analysis and simulation of network data. Journal of Statistical Software 24, 1548 â 1557. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Handel, S., Todd, S. K. & Zoidis, A. M. ( 2009 ). Rhythmic structure in humpback whale ( Megaptera novaeangliae ) songs: Preliminary implications for song production and perception. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 125, EL225 â EL230. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Handel, S., Todd, S. K. & Zoidis, A. M. ( 2012 ). Hierarchical and rhythmic organization in the songs of humpback whales ( Megaptera novaeangliae ). Bioacoustics 21, 141 â 156. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Harley, H. E. ( 2008 ). Whistle discrimination and categorization by the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin ( Tursiops truncatus ): a review of the signature whistle framework and a perceptual test. Behavioural Processes 77, 243 â 268. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Harnad, S. R. ( 1990 ). Categorical Perception: The Groundwork of Cognition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Harrington, F. H., Asa, C. S., Mech, L. & Boitani, L. ( 2003 ). Wolf communication. In Wolves: Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation, pp. 66 â 103. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hartley, R. S. & Suthers, R. A. ( 1989 ). Airflow and pressure during canary song: direct evidence for miniâ breaths. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 165, 15 â 26. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hartshorne, C. ( 1956 ). The monotonyâ threshold in singing birds. The Auk 73, 176 â 192. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hartshorne, C. ( 1973 ). Born to Sing. An Interpretation and World Survey of Bird Songs. Indiana University Press, London. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hasselquist, D., Bensch, S. & Von Schantz, T. ( 1996 ). Correlation between male song repertoire, extraâ pair paternity and offspring survival in the great reed warbler. Nature 381, 229 â 232. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hauser, M. D., Agnetta, B. & Perez, C. ( 1998 ). Orienting asymmetries in rhesus monkeys: the effect of timeâ domain changes on acoustic perception. Animal Behaviour 56, 41 â 47. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N. & Fitch, W. ( 2002 ). The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298, 1569 â 1579. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hausser, J. & Strimmer, K. ( 2009 ). Entropy inference and the Jamesâ Stein estimator, with application to nonlinear gene association networks. The Journal of Machine Learning Research 10, 1469 â 1484. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hayes, S. A., Kumar, A., Costa, D. P., Mellinger, D. K., Harvey, J. T., Southall, B. L. & Le Boeuf, B. J. ( 2004 ). Evaluating the function of the male harbour seal, Phoca vitulina, roar through playback experiments. Animal Behaviour 67, 1133 â 1139. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hebets, E. A. & Papaj, D. R. ( 2005 ). Complex signal function: developing a framework of testable hypotheses. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 57, 197 â 214. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Helble, T. A., Ierley, G. R., Gerald, L., Roch, M. A. & Hildebrand, J. A. ( 2012 ). A generalized powerâ law detection algorithm for humpback whale vocalizations. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 131, 2682 â 2699. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Henderson, E. E., Hildebrand, J. A. & Smith, M. H. ( 2011 ). Classification of behavior using vocalizations of Pacific whiteâ sided dolphins ( Lagenorhynchus obliquidens ). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 130, 557 â 567. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Henry, K. S., Gall, M. D., Bidelman, G. M. & Lucas, J. R. ( 2011 ). Songbirds tradeoff auditory frequency resolution and temporal resolution. Journal of Comparative Physiology, Part A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 197, 351 â 359. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Henry, K. S. & Lucas, J. R. ( 2010 ). Habitatâ related differences in the frequency selectivity of auditory filters in songbirds. Functional Ecology 24, 614 â 624. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Herzing, D. L. ( 1996 ). Vocalizations and associated underwater behavior of freeâ ranging Atlantic spotted dolphins, Stenella frontalis and bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. Aquatic Mammals 22, 61 â 80. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Holveck, M., Castro, D., Vieira, A. C., Lachlan, R. F., Ten Cate, C. & Riebel, K. ( 2008 ). Accuracy of song syntax learning and singing consistency signal early condition in zebra finches. Behavioral Ecology 19, 1267 â 1281. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Horning, C. L., Beecher, M. D., Stoddard, P. K. & Campbell, S. E. ( 1993 ). Song perception in the song sparrow: importance of different parts of the song in song type classification. Ethology 94, 46 â 58. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hulse, S. H. ( 2002 ). Auditory scene analysis in animal communication. Advances in the Study of Behavior 31, 163 â 200. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Humphries, M. D. & Gurney, K. ( 2008 ). Network â smallâ worldâ nessâ : a quantitative method for determining canonical network equivalence. PLoS ONE 3, e0002051. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ilany, A., Barocas, A., Kam, M., Ilany, T. & Geffen, E. ( 2013 ). The energy cost of singing in wild rock hyrax males: evidence for an index signal. Animal Behaviour 85, 995 â 1001. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ilany, A., Barocas, A., Koren, L., Kam, M. & Geffen, E. ( 2011 ). Do singing rock hyraxes exploit conspecific calls to gain attention? PLoS ONE 6, e28612. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Irwin, D. E. ( 2000 ). Song variation in an avian ring species. Evolution 54, 998 â 1010. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Isaac, D. & Marler, P. ( 1963 ). Ordering of sequences of singing behaviour of mistle thrushes in relationship to timing. Animal Behaviour 11, 179 â 188. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Janik, V. M. ( 1999 ). Pitfalls in the categorization of behaviour: a comparison of dolphin whistle classification methods. Animal Behaviour 57, 133 â 143. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Janik, V. M., King, S. L., Sayigh, L. S. & Wells, R. S. ( 2013 ). Identifying signature whistles from recordings of groups of unrestrained bottlenose dolphins ( Tursiops truncatus ). Marine Mammal Science 29, 109 â 122. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Janik, V. M., Sayigh, L. & Wells, R. ( 2006 ). Signature whistle shape conveys identity information to bottlenose dolphins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103, 8293 â 8297. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Jansen, D. A., Cant, M. A. & Manser, M. B. ( 2012 ). Segmental concatenation of individual signatures and conâ text cues in banded mongoose ( Mungos mungo ) close calls. BMC Biology 10, 97. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ji, A., Johnson, M. T., Walsh, E. J., Mcgee, J. & Armstrong, D. L. ( 2013 ). Discrimination of individual tigers ( Panthera tigris ) from long distance roars. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 133, 1762 â 1769. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Jin, D. Z. ( 2009 ). Generating variable birdsong syllable sequences with branching chain networks in avian premotor nucleus HVC. Physical Review E 80, 051902. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Jin, D. Z. & Kozhevnikov, A. A. ( 2011 ). A compact statistical model of the song syntax in Bengalese finch. PLoS Computational Biology 7, e1001108. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Johnson, K. ( 2004 ). Gold’s theorem and cognitive science. Philosophy of Science 71, 571 â 592. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Jones, A. E., Ten Cate, C. & Bijleveld, C. C. ( 2001 ). The interobserver reliability of scoring sonagrams by eye: a study on methods, illustrated on zebra finch songs. Animal Behaviour 62, 791 â 801. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Jouventin, P., Aubin, T. & Lengagne, T. ( 1999 ). Finding a parent in a king penguin colony: the acoustic system of individual recognition. Animal Behaviour 57, 1175 â 1183. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Jurafsky, D. & Martin, J. H. ( 2000 ). Speech and Language Processing: An Introduction to Natural Language Processing, Computational Linguistics, and Speech Recognition. Prenticeâ Hall, New Jersey. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kakishita, Y., Sasahara, K., Nishino, T., Takahasi, M. & Okanoya, K. ( 2009 ). Ethological data mining: an automataâ based approach to extract behavioral units and rules. Data Mining and Knowledge Discovery 18, 446 â 471. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kanwal, J. S., Matsumura, S., Ohlemiller, K. & Suga, N. ( 1994 ). Analysis of acoustic elements and syntax in communication sounds emitted by mustached bats. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 96, 1229 â 1254. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kaplan, D. ( 1978 ). Dthat. Syntax and Semantics 9, 221 â 243. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kashtan, N., Itzkovitz, S., Milo, R. & Alon, U. ( 2002 ). Mfinder tool guide. Technical Report. Department of Molecular Cell Biology and Computer Science and Applied Mathematics, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Katahira, K., Suzuki, K., Okanoya, K. & Okada, M. ( 2011 ). Complex sequencing rules of birdsong can be explained by simple hidden Markov processes. PLoS ONE 6, e24516. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Katz, S. ( 1987 ). Estimation of probabilities from sparse data for the language model component of a speech recognizer. IEEE Transactions on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing 35, 400 â 401. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kershenbaum, A. ( 2013 ). Entropy rate as a measure of animal vocal complexity. Bioacoustics 23, 195 â 208. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kershenbaum, A., Bowles, A. E., Freeberg, T. M., Jin, D. Z., Lameira, A. R. & Bohn, K. ( 2014 ). Animal vocal sequences: not the Markov chains we thought they were. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 281, 20141370. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kershenbaum, A., Ilany, A., Blaustein, L. & Geffen, E. ( 2012 ). Syntactic structure and geographical dialects in the songs of male rock hyraxes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 279, 2974 â 2981. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kershenbaum, A. & Roch, M. A. ( 2013 ). An image processing based paradigm for the extraction of tonal sounds in cetacean communications. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 134, 4435 â 4445. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kershenbaum, A., Sayigh, L. S. & Janik, V. M. ( 2013 ). The encoding of individual identity in dolphin signature whistles: how much information is needed? PLoS ONE 8, e77671. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kipper, S., Mundry, R., Sommer, C., Hultsch, H. & Todt, D. ( 2006 ). Song repertoire size is correlated with body measures and arrival date in common nightingales, Luscinia megarhynchos. Animal Behaviour 71, 211 â 217. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kitchen, D. M. ( 2004 ). Alpha male black howler monkey responses to loud calls: effect of numeric odds, male companion behaviour and reproductive investment. Animal Behaviour 67, 125 â 139. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Klinck, H., Kindermann, L. & Boebel, O. ( 2008 ). Detection of leopard seal ( Hydrurga leptonyx ) vocalizations using the Envelopeâ Spectrogram Technique (TEST) in combination with a Hidden Markov model. Canadian Acoustics 36, 118 â 124. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kogan, J. A. & Margoliash, D. ( 1998 ). Automated recognition of bird song elements from continuous recordings using dynamic time warping and hidden Markov models: a comparative study. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 103, 2185 â 2196. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kolodny, O., Lotem, A. & Edelman, S. ( 2014 ). Learning a generative probabilistic grammar of experience: a processâ level model of language acquisition. Cognitive Science 38, 1 â 41. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Koren, L. & Geffen, E. ( 2009 ). Complex call in male rock hyrax ( Procavia capensis ): a multiâ information distributing channel. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63, 581 â 590. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Koren, L. & Geffen, E. ( 2011 ). Individual identity is communicated through multiple pathways in male rock hyrax ( Procavia capensis ) songs. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65, 675 â 684. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Krams, I., Krama, T., Freeberg, T. M., Kullberg, C. & Lucas, J. R. ( 2012 ). Linking social complexity and vocal complexity: a parid perspective. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences 367, 1879 â 1891. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Krebs, J. ( 1976 ). Habituation and song repertoires in the great tit. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 1, 215 â 227. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Krebs, J. R., Ashcroft, R. & Orsdol, K. V. ( 1981 ). Song matching in the great tit Parus major L. Animal Behaviour 29, 918 â 923. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kroodsma, D. E. ( 1985 ). Development and use of two song forms by the Eastern Phoebe. The Wilson Bulletin 97, 21 â 29. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kroodsma, D. E. ( 1989 ). Suggested experimental designs for song playbacks. Animal Behaviour 37, 600 â 609. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kroodsma, D. E. ( 1990 ). Patterns in songbird singing behaviour: Hartshorne vindicated. Animal Behaviour 39, 994 â 996. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lachlan, R. F., Verzijden, M. N., Bernard, C. S., Jonker, P., Koese, B., Jaarsma, S., Spoor, W., Slater, P. J. & Ten Cate, C. ( 2013 ). The progressive loss of syntactical structure in bird song along an island colonization chain. Current Biology 19, 1896 â 1901. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lambrechts, M. & Dhondt, A. ( 1987 ). Differences in singing performance between male great tits. Ardea 75, 43 â 52. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lambrechts, M. & Dhondt, A. A. ( 1988 ). The antiâ exhaustion hypothesis: a new hypothesis to explain song performance and song switching in the great tit. Animal Behaviour 36, 327 â 334. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lammers, M. O. & Au, W. W. ( 2003 ). Directionality in the whistles of Hawaiian spinner dolphins ( Stenella longirostris ): a signal feature to cue direction of movement? Marine Mammal Science 19, 249 â 264. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Larson, K. A. ( 2004 ). Advertisement call complexity in northern leopard frogs, Rana pipiens. Copeia 2004, 676 â 682. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lee, K. & Hon, H. ( 1989 ). Speakerâ independent phone recognition using hidden Markov models. IEEE Transactions on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing 37, 1641 â 1648. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lehongre, K., Aubin, T., Robin, S. & Del Negro, C. ( 2008 ). Individual signature in canary songs: contribution of multiple levels of song structure. Ethology 114, 425 â 435. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lemasson, A., Ouattara, K., Bouchet, H. & Zuberbühler, K. ( 2010 ). Speed of call delivery is related to context and caller identity in Campbell’s monkey males. Naturwissenschaften 97, 1023 â 1027. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lengagne, T., Aubin, T., Jouventin, P. & Lauga, J. ( 2000 ). Perceptual salience of individually distinctive features in the calls of adult king penguins. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 107, 508 â 516. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lengagne, T., Lauga, J. & Aubin, T. ( 2001 ). Intraâ syllabic acoustic signatures used by the king penguin in parentâ chick recognition: an experimental approach. The Journal of Experimental Biology 204, 663 â 672. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lipkind, D., Marcus, G. F., Bemis, D. K., Sasahara, K., Jacoby, N., Takahasi, M., Suzuki, K., Feher, O., Ravbar, P. & Okanoya, K. ( 2013 ). Stepwise acquisition of vocal combinatorial capacity in songbirds and human infants. Nature 498, 104 â 108. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Liu, I. A., Lohr, B., Olsen, B. & Greenberg, R. ( 2008 ). Macrogeographic vocal variation in subspecies of swamp sparrow. The Condor 110, 102 â 109. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lohr, B., Ashby, S. & Wakamiya, S. M. ( 2013 ). The function of song types and song components in Grasshopper Sparrows ( Ammodramus savannarum ). Behaviour 150, 1085 â 1106. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lynch, K. S., Stanely Rand, A., Ryan, M. J. & Wilczynski, W. ( 2005 ). Plasticity in female mate choice associated with changing reproductive states. Animal Behaviour 69, 689 â 699. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Macedonia, J. M. ( 1990 ). What is communicated in the antipredator calls of lemurs: evidence from playback experiments with ringtailed and ruffed lemurs. Ethology 86, 177 â 190. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mahurin, E. J. & Freeberg, T. M. ( 2009 ). Chickâ aâ dee call variation in Carolina chickadees and recruiting flockmates to food. Behavioral Ecology 20, 111 â 116. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Makeig, S., Debener, S., Onton, J. & Delorme, A. ( 2004 ). Mining eventâ related brain dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, 204 â 210. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mallat, S. ( 1999 ). A Wavelet Tour of Signal Processing. Academic Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Manning, C. D., Raghavan, P. & Schütze, H. ( 2008 ). Introduction to Information Retrieval. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Manser, M. B. ( 2001 ). The acoustic structure of suricates’ alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 268, 2315 â 2324. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Marler, P. ( 1977 ). The structure of animal communication sounds. In Recognition of Complex Acoustic Signals (ed. T. H. Bullock ), pp. 17 â 35. Springerâ Verlag, Berlin. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Marler, P. & Pickert, R. ( 1984 ). Speciesâ universal microstructure in the learned song of the swamp sparrow ( Melospiza georgiana ). Animal Behaviour 32, 673 â 689. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Marler, P. & Sherman, V. ( 1985 ). Innate differences in singing behaviour of sparrows reared in isolation from adult conspecific song. Animal Behaviour 33, 57 â 71. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Martin, W. P. ( 1972 ). Evolution of vocalization in the genus Bufo. In Evolution in the GenusBufo (ed. W. F. Blair ), pp. 37 â 70. University of Texas Press, Austin. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Martin, W. & Flandrin, P. ( 1985 ). Wignerâ Ville spectral analysis of nonstationary processes. IEEE Transactions on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing 33, 1461 â 1470. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Martin, W. F. & Gans, C. ( 1972 ). Muscular control of the vocal tract during release signaling in the toad Bufo valliceps. Journal of Morphology 137, 1 â 27. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Martinich, A. & Sosa, D. ( 2013 ). The Philosophy of Language. Oxford University Press, Oxford. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mathevon, N. & Aubin, T. ( 2001 ). Soundâ based speciesâ specific recognition in the blackcap Sylvia atricapilla shows high tolerance to signal modifications. Behaviour 138, 511 â 524. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mathevon, N., Aubin, T., Vielliard, J., Da Silva, M., Sebe, F. & Boscolo, D. ( 2008 ). Singing in the rain forest: how a tropical bird song transfers information. PLoS ONE 3, e1580. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mathevon, N., Koralek, A., Weldele, M., Glickman, S. E. & Theunissen, F. E. ( 2010 ). What the hyena’s laugh tells: sex, age, dominance and individual signature in the giggling call of Crocuta crocuta. BMC Ecology 10, 9. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Matthews, J., Rendell, L., Gordon, J. & Macdonald, D. ( 1999 ). A review of frequency and time parameters of cetacean tonal calls. Bioacoustics 10, 47 â 71. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mccowan, B., Doyle, L. R. & Hanser, S. F. ( 2002 ). Using information theory to assess the diversity, complexity, and development of communicative repertoires. Journal of Comparative Psychology 116, 166 â 172. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mccowan, B., Hanser, S. F. & Doyle, L. R. ( 1999 ). Quantitative tools for comparing animal communication systems: information theory applied to bottlenose dolphin whistle repertoires. Animal Behaviour 57, 409 â 419. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mccowan, B. & Reiss, D. ( 1995 ). Quantitative comparison of whistle repertoires from captive adult bottlenose dolphins (Delphinidae, Tursiops truncatus ): a reâ evaluation of the signature whistle hypothesis. Ethology 100, 194 â 209. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mcshea, D. W. ( 1991 ). Complexity and evolution: what everybody knows. Biology and Philosophy 6, 303 â 324. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mcshea, D. W. ( 2009 ). The evolution of complexity without natural selection, a possible largeâ scale trend of the fourth kind. Paleobiology 31, 146 â 156. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mennill, D. J. & Ratcliffe, L. M. ( 2004 ). Overlapping and matching in the song contests of blackâ capped chickadees. Animal Behaviour 67, 441 â 450. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mennill, D. J., Ratcliffe, L. M. & Boag, P. T. ( 2002 ). Female eavesdropping on male song contests in songbirds. Science 296, 873. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mercado, E. I. & Handel, S. ( 2012 ). Understanding the structure of humpback whale songs (L). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 132, 2947 â 2950. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mercado, E. I., Herman, L. M. & Pack, A. A. ( 2003 ). Stereotypical sound patterns in humpback whale songs: usage and function. Aquatic Mammals 29, 37 â 52. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mercado, E. I., Schneider, J. N., Pack, A. A. & Herman, L. M. ( 2010 ). Sound production by singing humpback whales. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 127, 2678 â 2691. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Miller, C. T. & Bee, M. A. ( 2012 ). Receiver psychology turns 20: is it time for a broader approach? Animal Behaviour 83, 331 â 343. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Miller, C. T. & Cohen, Y. E. ( 2010 ). Vocalizations as auditory objects: behavior and neurophysiology. In Primate Neuroethology (eds M. L. Platt and A. A. Ghazanfar ), pp. 237 â 255. Oxford University Press, Oxford. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Miller, P. J., Samarra, F. I. & Perthuison, A. D. ( 2007 ). Caller sex and orientation influence spectral characteristics of â twoâ voiceâ stereotyped calls produced by freeâ ranging killer whales. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 121, 3932 â 3937. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Millikan, R. G. ( 2004 ). Varieties of Meaning: The 2002 Jean Nicod Lectures. MIT Press, Boston. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Milo, R., Shenâ Orr, S., Itzkovitz, S., Kashtan, N., Chklovskii, D. & Alon, U. ( 2002 ). Network motifs: simple building blocks of complex networks. Science 298, 824 â 827. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mitani, J. C., Hunley, K. & Murdoch, M. ( 1999 ). Geographic variation in the calls of wild chimpanzees: a reassessment. American Journal of Primatology 47, 133 â 151. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mitchell, M. ( 2009 ). Complexity: A Guided Tour. Oxford University Press, Oxford. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Moore, B. C. & Moore, B. C. ( 2003 ). An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing. Academic Press, San Diego. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mulligan, J. A. ( 1966 ). Singing Behavior and its Development in the Song Sparrow, Melospiza Melodia, University of California Publications in Zoology, Volume 81. pp. 1 â 76. University of California Press, Berkeley. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mumford, D. & Desolneux, A. ( 2010 ). Pattern Theory: The Stochastic Analysis of Realâ World Signals. A. K. Peters Ltd., Natick. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Munoz, N. E. & Blumstein, D. T. ( 2012 ). Multisensory perception in uncertain environments. Behavioral Ecology 23, 457 â 462. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Murray, S. O., Mercado, E. I. & Roitblat, H. L. ( 1998 ). Characterizing the graded structure of false killer whale ( Pseudorca crassidens ) vocalizations. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 104, 1679 â 1688. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Narins, P. M., Lewis, E. R. & Mcclelland, B. E. ( 2000 ). Hyperextended call note repertoire of the endemic Madagascar treefrog Boophis madagascariensis (Rhacophoridae). Journal of Zoology 250, 283 â 298. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Narins, P. M., Reichman, O., Jarvis, J. U. & Lewis, E. R. ( 1992 ). Seismic signal transmission between burrows of the Cape moleâ rat, Georychus capensis. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 170, 13 â 21. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Nelson, D. A. & Marler, P. ( 1989 ). Categorical perception of a natural stimulus continuum: birdsong. Science 244, 976 â 978. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Nelson, D. A. & Poesel, A. ( 2007 ). Segregation of information in a complex acoustic signal: individual and dialect identity in whiteâ crowned sparrow song. Animal Behaviour 74, 1073 â 1084. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Nettle, D. ( 1999 ). Language variation and the evolution of societies. In The Evolution of Culture: An Interdisciplinary View (eds R. I. M. Dunbar, C. Knight and C. Power ), pp. 214 â 227. Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Neubauer, R. L. ( 1999 ). Superâ normal length song preferences of female zebra finches ( Taeniopygia guttata ) and a theory of the evolution of bird song. Evolutionary Ecology 13, 365 â 380. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Nevo, E., Heth, G., Beiles, A. & Frankenberg, E. ( 1987 ). Geographic dialects in blind mole rats: role of vocal communication in active speciation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 84, 3312 â 3315. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Newman, M. E. ( 2003 ). The structure and function of complex networks. SIAM Review 45, 167 â 256. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Newman, M. ( 2009 ). Networks: An Introduction. Oxford University Press, Oxford. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Nityananda, V. & Bee, M. A. ( 2011 ). Finding your mate at a cocktail party: frequency separation promotes auditory stream segregation of concurrent voices in multiâ species frog choruses. PLoS ONE 6, e21191. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Notman, H. & Rendall, D. ( 2005 ). Contextual variation in chimpanzee pant hoots and its implications for referential communication. Animal Behaviour 70, 177 â 190. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Nowicki, S. & Marler, P. ( 1988 ). How do birds sing? Music Perception 5, 391 â 426. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Nowicki, S. & Nelson, D. A. ( 1990 ). Defining natural categories in acoustic signals: comparison of three methods applied to â chickâ aâ deeâ call notes. Ethology 86, 89 â 101. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ohms, V. R., Escudero, P., Lammers, K. & Ten Cate, C. ( 2012 ). Zebra finches and Dutch adults exhibit the same cue weighting bias in vowel perception. Animal Cognition 15, 155 â 161. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Oleson, E. M., Wiggins, S. M. & Hildebrand, J. A. ( 2007 ). Temporal separation of blue whale call types on a southern California feeding ground. Animal Behaviour 74, 881 â 894. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Oppenheim, A. V. & Schafer, R. W. ( 2004 ). From frequency to quefrency: a history of the cepstrum. IEEE Signal Processing Magazine 21, 95 â 106. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Opsahl, T., Agneessens, F. & Skvoretz, J. ( 2010 ). Node centrality in weighted networks: generalizing degree and shortest paths. Social Networks 32, 245 â 251. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Oswald, J. N., Rankin, S., Barlow, J. & Lammers, M. O. ( 2007 ). A tool for realâ time acoustic species identification of delphinid whistles. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 122, 587 â 595. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Otter, K., Mcgregor, P. K., Terry, A. M., Burford, F. R., Peake, T. M. & Dabelsteen, T. ( 1999 ). Do female great tits ( Parus major ) assess males by eavesdropping? A field study using interactive song playback. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences 266, 1305 â 1309. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Owren, M. J., Rendall, D. & Ryan, M. J. ( 2010 ). Redefining animal signaling: influence versus information in communication. Biology & Philosophy 25, 755 â 780. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Pack, A. A., Herman, L. M., Hoffmannâ Kuhnt, M. & Branstetter, B. K. ( 2002 ). The object behind the echo: dolphins ( Tursiops truncatus ) perceive object shape globally through echolocation. Behavioural Processes 58, 1 â 26. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Page, S. E. ( 2010 ). Diversity and Complexity. Princeton University Press, Princeton. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Parsons, S., Riskin, D. K. & Hermanson, J. W. ( 2010 ). Echolocation call production during aerial and terrestrial locomotion by New Zealand’s enigmatic lesser shortâ tailed bat, Mystacina tuberculata. The Journal of Experimental Biology 213, 551 â 557. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Partan, S. & Marler, P. ( 1999 ). Communication goes multimodal. Science 283, 1272 â 1273. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Paulus, J., Müller, M. & Klapuri, A. ( 2010 ). State of the art report: audioâ based music structure analysis. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Music Information Retrieval (ISMIR), Utrecht. pp. 625 â 636. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Payne, K. B., Langbauer, W. R. Jr. & Thomas, E. M. ( 1986 ). Infrasonic calls of the Asian elephant ( Elephas maximus ). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 18, 297 â 301. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Payne, R. S. & Mcvay, S. ( 1971 ). Songs of humpback whales. Science 173, 585 â 597. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Payne, K., Tyack, P. & Payne, R. ( 1983 ). Progressive changes in the songs of humpback whales ( Megaptera novaeangliae ): a detailed analysis of two seasons in Hawaii. In Communication and Behavior of Whales, pp. 9 â 57. Westview Press, Boulder. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Peterson, G. E. & Barney, H. L. ( 1952 ). Control methods used in a study of the vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 24, 175 â 184. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Picone, J. W. ( 1993 ). Signal modeling techniques in speech recognition. Proceedings of the IEEE 81, 1215 â 1247. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Pitcher, B. J., Harcourt, R. G. & Charrier, I. ( 2012 ). Individual identity encoding and environmental constraints in vocal recognition of pups by Australian sea lion mothers. Animal Behaviour 83, 681 â 690. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Podos, J. ( 1997 ). A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (Passeriformes: Emberizidae). Evolution 51, 537 â 551. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Podos, J., Peters, S., Rudnicky, T., Marler, P. & Nowicki, S. ( 1992 ). The organization of song repertoires in song sparrows: themes and variations. Ethology 90, 89 â 106. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Pollard, K. A. & Blumstein, D. T. ( 2011 ). Social group size predicts the evolution of individuality. Current Biology 21, 413 â 417. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Pollard, K. A. & Blumstein, D. T. ( 2012 ). Evolving communicative complexity: insights from rodents and beyond. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences 367, 1869 â 1878. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Poole, J. H. ( 1999 ). Signals and assessment in African elephants: evidence from playback experiments. Animal Behaviour 58, 185 â 193. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Putnam, H. ( 1975 ). The meaning of â meaningâ . Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science 7, 131 â 193. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Quick, N. J. & Janik, V. M. ( 2012 ). Bottlenose dolphins exchange signature whistles when meeting at sea. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 279, 2539 â 2545. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Quine, W. V. O. ( 1960 ). Word and Object. MIT Press, Cambridge. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | R Development Team ( 2012 ). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rabiner, L. R. ( 1989 ). A tutorial on hidden Markov models and selected applications in speech recognition. Proceedings of the IEEE 77, 257 â 286. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Raemaekers, J. J., Raemaekers, P. M. & Haimoff, E. H. ( 1984 ). Loud calls of the gibbon ( Hylobates lar ): repertoire, organisation and context. Behaviour 91, 146 â 189. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ragge, D. & Reynolds, W. ( 1988 ). The songs and taxonomy of the grasshoppers of the Chorthippus biguttulus group in the Iberian Peninsula (Orthoptera: Acrididae). Journal of Natural History 22, 897 â 929. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Randall, J. A. ( 1989 ). Individual footdrumming signatures in bannerâ tailed kangaroo rats Dipodomys spectabilis. Animal Behaviour 38, 620 â 630. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Randall, J. A. ( 1997 ). Speciesâ specific footdrumming in kangaroo rats: Dipodomys ingens, D. deserti, D. spectabilis. Animal Behaviour 54, 1167 â 1175. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Recanzone, G. H. & Sutter, M. L. ( 2008 ). The biological basis of audition. Annual Review of Psychology 59, 119 â 142. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Remez, R. E., Rubin, P. E., Berns, S. M., Pardo, J. S. & Lang, J. M. ( 1994 ). On the perceptual organization of speech. Psychological Review 101, 129. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ribeiro, S., Cecchi, G. A., Magnasco, M. O. & Mello, C. V. ( 1998 ). Toward a song code: evidence for a syllabic representation in the canary brain. Neuron 21, 359 â 371. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Richards, D. G. ( 1981 ). Alerting and message components in songs of rufousâ sided towhees. Behaviour 76, 223 â 249. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Riebel, K. & Slater, P. ( 2003 ). Temporal variation in male chaffinch song depends on the singer and the song type. Behaviour 140, 269 â 288. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Riede, T., Bronson, E., Hatzikirou, H. & Zuberbühler, K. ( 2005 ). Vocal production mechanisms in a nonâ human primate: morphological data and a model. Journal of Human Evolution 48, 85 â 96. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Riede, T. & Fitch, T. ( 1999 ). Vocal tract length and acoustics of vocalization in the domestic dog ( Canis familiaris ). The Journal of Experimental Biology 202, 2859 â 2867. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ripley, B. D. ( 2007 ). Pattern Recognition and Neural Networks. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Robinson, J. G. ( 1979 ). An analysis of the organization of vocal communication in the titi monkey Callicebus moloch. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 49, 381 â 405. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Robisson, P., Aubin, T. & Bremond, J. ( 1993 ). Individuality in the voice of the emperor penguin Aptenodytes forsteri: adaptation to a noisy environment. Ethology 94, 279 â 290. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rootâ Gutteridge, H., Bencsik, M., Chebli, M., Gentle, L. K., Terrellâ Nield, C., Bourit, A. & Yarnell, R. W. ( 2014 ). Identifying individual wild Eastern grey wolves ( Canis lupus lycaon ) using fundamental frequency and amplitude of howls. Bioacoustics 23, 55 â 66. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rothenberg, D., Roeske, T. C., Voss, H. U., Naguib, M. & Tchernichovski, O. ( 2013 ). Investigation of musicality in birdsong. Hearing Research 308, 71 â 83. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ruxton, G. D. & Schaefer, H. M. ( 2011 ). Resolving current disagreements and ambiguities in the terminology of animal communication. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 24, 2574 â 2585. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Saar, S. & Mitra, P. P. ( 2008 ). A technique for characterizing the development of rhythms in bird song. PLoS ONE 3, e1461. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Salmi, R., Hammerschmidt, K. & Doranâ Sheehy, D. M. ( 2013 ). Western gorilla vocal repertoire and contextual use of vocalizations. Ethology 119, 831 â 847. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Sasahara, K., Cody, M. L., Cohen, D. & Taylor, C. E. ( 2012 ). Structural design principles of complex bird songs: a networkâ based approach. PLoS ONE 7, e44436. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Saunders, A. A. ( 1951 ). The song of the Song Sparrow. The Wilson Bulletin 63, 99 â 109. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Sayigh, L. S., Esch, H. C., Wells, R. S. & Janik, V. M. ( 2007 ). Facts about signature whistles of bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. Animal Behaviour 74, 1631 â 1642. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Sayigh, L., Quick, N., Hastie, G. & Tyack, P. ( 2012 ). Repeated call types in shortâ finned pilot whales, Globicephala macrorhynchus. Marine Mammal Science 29, 312 â 324. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Sayigh, L. S., Tyack, P. L., Wells, R. S., Solow, A. R., Scott, M. D. & Irvine, A. B. ( 1999 ). Individual recognition in wild bottlenose dolphins: a field test using playback experiments. Animal Behaviour 57, 41 â 50. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Schel, A. M., Candiotti, A. & Zuberbühler, K. ( 2010 ). Predatorâ deterring alarm call sequences in Guereza colobus monkeys are meaningful to conspecifics. Animal Behaviour 80, 799 â 808. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Schel, A. M., Tranquilli, S. & Zuberbühler, K. ( 2009 ). The alarm call system of two species of blackâ andâ white colobus monkeys ( Colobus polykomos and Colobus guereza ). Journal of Comparative Psychology 123, 136 â 150. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Schreiber, F. & Schwöbbermeyer, H. ( 2005 ). MAVisto: a tool for the exploration of network motifs. Bioinformatics 21, 3572 â 3574. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Schulz, T. M., Whitehead, H., Gero, S. & Rendell, L. ( 2008 ). Overlapping and matching of codas in vocal interactions between sperm whales: insights into communication function. Animal Behaviour 76, 1977 â 1988. | |
dc.owningcollname | Interdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed |
Files in this item
Remediation of Harmful Language
The University of Michigan Library aims to describe its collections in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in them. We encourage you to Contact Us anonymously if you encounter harmful or problematic language in catalog records or finding aids. More information about our policies and practices is available at Remediation of Harmful Language.
Accessibility
If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.