Show simple item record

Differences in the Evaluation of Generic Statements About Human and Non‐Human Categories

dc.contributor.authorTasimi, Arber
dc.contributor.authorGelman, Susan A.
dc.contributor.authorCimpian, Andrei
dc.contributor.authorKnobe, Joshua
dc.date.accessioned2017-10-05T18:18:11Z
dc.date.available2018-12-03T15:34:03Zen
dc.date.issued2017-09
dc.identifier.citationTasimi, Arber; Gelman, Susan A.; Cimpian, Andrei; Knobe, Joshua (2017). "Differences in the Evaluation of Generic Statements About Human and Non‐Human Categories." Cognitive Science 41(7): 1934-1957.
dc.identifier.issn0364-0213
dc.identifier.issn1551-6709
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/138302
dc.description.abstractGeneric statements (e.g., “Birds lay eggs”) express generalizations about categories. Current theories suggest that people should be especially inclined to accept generics that involve threatening information. However, previous tests of this claim have focused on generics about non‐human categories, which raises the question of whether this effect applies as readily to human categories. In Experiment 1, adults were more likely to accept generics involving a threatening (vs. a non‐threatening) property for artifacts, but this negativity bias did not also apply to human categories. Experiment 2 examined an alternative hypothesis for this result, and Experiments 3 and 4 served as conceptual replications of the first experiment. Experiment 5 found that even preschoolers apply generics differently for humans and artifacts. Finally, Experiment 6 showed that these effects reflect differences between human and non‐human categories more generally, as adults showed a negativity bias for categories of non‐human animals, but not for categories of humans. These findings suggest the presence of important, early‐emerging domain differences in people’s judgments about generics.
dc.publisherUniversity of Massachusetts
dc.publisherWiley Periodicals, Inc.
dc.subject.otherGeneric language
dc.subject.otherConcepts
dc.subject.otherCognitive development
dc.subject.otherPsychological essentialism
dc.titleDifferences in the Evaluation of Generic Statements About Human and Non‐Human Categories
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollow
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelNeurosciences
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelHealth Sciences
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Reviewed
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttps://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/138302/1/cogs12440.pdf
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttps://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/138302/2/cogs12440_am.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/cogs.12440
dc.identifier.sourceCognitive Science
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKelemen, D., & Rosset, E. ( 2009 ). The human function compunction: Teleological explanation in adults. Cognition, 111, 138 – 143. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.001
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDe Freitas, J., & Cikara, M. ( 2016 ). Deep down my enemy is good: Thinking about the true self reduces intergroup bias. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDevine, P. G. ( 1989 ). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5 – 18. doi: 10.1037/0022‐3514.56.1.5
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEidson, R. C., & Coley, J. D. ( 2014 ). Not so fast: Reassessing gender essentialism in young adults. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15 ( 2 ), 382 – 392. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2013.763810
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEpley, N., Morewedge, C. K., & Keysar, B. ( 2004 ). Perspective taking in children and adults: Equivalent egocentricism but differential correction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 760 – 768. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2004.02.002
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFrederick, S. ( 2005 ). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19, 25 – 42. doi: 10.1257/089533005775196732
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A. ( 2003 ). The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. New York, NY: Oxford University Press
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A. ( 2004 ). Psychological essentialism in children. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 404 – 409. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.001
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., Taylor, M. G., & Nguyen, S. P. ( 2004 ). Mother–child conversations about gender: Understanding the acquisition of essentialist beliefs. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69, 1 – 127
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHeyman, G. D., & Dweck, C. S. ( 1998 ). Children’s thinking about traits: Implications for judgments of the self and others. Child Development, 69, 391 – 403. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06197.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHirschfeld, L. A., & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.) ( 1994 ). Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKahneman, D. ( 2011 ). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLeslie, S. J. ( 2008 ). Generics: Cognition and acquisition. Philosophical Review, 117, 1 – 47. doi: 10.1215/00318108‐2007‐023
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLeslie, S. J. (in press). The original sin of cognition: Fear, prejudice, and generalization. Journal of Philosophy.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLyons, J. ( 1977 ). Semantics: I. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNewman, G. E., Bloom, P., & Knobe, J. ( 2014 ). Value judgments and the true self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 203 – 216. doi: 10.1177/0146167213508791
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNewman, G. E., De Freitas, J., & Knobe, J. ( 2015 ). Beliefs about the true self explain asymmetries based on moral judgment. Cognitive Science, 39, 96 – 125. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12134
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRhodes, M., & Brickman, D. ( 2011 ). The influence of competition on children’s social categories. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12, 194 – 221. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2010.535230
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRholes, W. S., & Ruble, D. N. ( 1986 ). Children’s impressions of other persons: The effect of temporal separation of behavioral information. Child Development, 57, 872 – 878. doi: 10.2307/1130364
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. ( 2001 ). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 296 – 320. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSloman, S. A. ( 1996 ). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3 – 22. doi: 10.1037/0033‐2909.119.1.3
dc.identifier.citedreferenceVaish, A., Grossmann, T., & Woodward, A. ( 2008 ). Not all emotions are created equal: The negativity bias in social‐emotional development. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 383 – 403. doi: 10.1037/0033‐2909.134.3.383
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAbrams, D., Rutland, A., Cameron, L., & Ferrell, J. ( 2007 ). Older but wilier: In‐group accountability and the development of subjective group dynamics. Developmental Psychology, 43, 134 – 148. doi: 10.1037/0012‐1649.43.1.134
dc.identifier.citedreferenceApfelbaum, E. P., Pauker, K., Ambady, N., Sommers, S. R., & Norton, M. I. ( 2008 ). Learning (not) to talk about race: When older children underperform in social categorization. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1513 – 1518. doi: 10.1037/a0012835
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBaumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. ( 2001 ). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323 – 370. doi: 10.1037/1089‐2680.5.4.323
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBoseovski, J. J. ( 2010 ). Evidence for “rose‐colored glasses”: An examination of the positivity bias in young children’s personality judgments. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 212 – 218. doi: 10.1111/j.1750‐8606.2010.00149.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBrandone, A. C., Gelman, S. A., & Hedglen, J. ( 2015 ). Children’s developing intuitions about the truth conditions and implications of novel generics versus quantified statements. Cognitive Science, 39, 711 – 738. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12176
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarlson, G. N. ( 1977 ). Reference to kinds in English. Doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarlson, G. N., & Pelletier, F. J. ( 1995 ). The generic book. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCimpian, A., Brandone, A. C., & Gelman, S. A. ( 2010 ). Generic statements require little evidence for acceptance but have powerful implications. Cognitive Science, 34, 1452 – 1482. doi: 10.1111/j.1551‐6709.2010.01126.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCimpian, A., & Cadena, C. ( 2010 ). Why are dunkels sticky? Preschoolers infer functionality and intentional creation for artifact properties learned from generic language. Cognition, 117, 62 – 68. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.06.011
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCimpian, A., & Markman, E. M. ( 2009 ). Information learned from generic language becomes central to children’s biological concepts: Evidence from their open‐ended explanations. Cognition, 113, 14 – 25. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.07.004
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCimpian, A., & Markman, E. M. ( 2011 ). The generic/nongeneric distinction influences how children interpret new information about social others. Child Development, 82, 471 – 492. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‐8624.2010.01525.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCimpian, A., & Steinberg, O. D. ( 2014 ). The inherence heuristic across development: Systematic differences between children’s and adults’ explanations for everyday facts. Cognitive Psychology, 75, 130 – 154. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2014.09.001
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCohen, A. ( 1999 ). Think generic!: The meaning and use of generic sentences. Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.