Show simple item record

Can supporting a cause decrease donations and happiness? The cause marketing paradox

dc.contributor.authorKrishna, Aradhna
dc.date.accessioned2018-02-05T16:27:02Z
dc.date.available2018-02-05T16:27:02Z
dc.date.issued2011-07
dc.identifier.citationKrishna, Aradhna (2011). "Can supporting a cause decrease donations and happiness? The cause marketing paradox." Journal of Consumer Psychology 21(3): 338-345.
dc.identifier.issn1057-7408
dc.identifier.issn1532-7663
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/141072
dc.description.abstractIn two laboratory and one pilot field study, we demonstrate that cause marketing, whereby firms link products with a cause and share proceeds with it, reduces charitable giving by consumers, even when it is costless to the consumer to buy on CM (versus not); further, instead of increasing total contribution to the cause, it can decrease it. Consumers appear to realize that participating in cause marketing is inherently more selfish than direct charitable donation, and are less happy if they substitute cause marketing for charitable giving. Our results suggest that egoistic and empathetic altruism may have different effects on happiness.
dc.publisherWiley Periodicals, Inc.
dc.publisherUniversity of Minnesota Press
dc.subject.otherHappiness
dc.subject.otherPro‐social
dc.subject.otherCause‐marketing
dc.subject.otherAltruism
dc.subject.otherCharity
dc.titleCan supporting a cause decrease donations and happiness? The cause marketing paradox
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollow
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPsychology
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciences
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Reviewed
dc.contributor.affiliationumDwight F. Benton Professor of Marketing at the Ross School of Business, University of Michigan, 701 Tappan St. Ann Arbor, MI 48109–1234, USA
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttps://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/141072/1/jcpy338.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1016/j.jcps.2011.02.001
dc.identifier.sourceJournal of Consumer Psychology
dc.identifier.citedreferenceW. Liu, J. Aaker. The happiness of giving: The time‐ask effect. Journal of Consumer Research. 2008; 35: 543 – 557 (October).
dc.identifier.citedreferenceR.B. Cialdini, M. Schaller, D. Houlihan, K. Arps, J. Fultz, A.L. Beaman. Empathy‐based helping: Is it selflessly or selfishly motivated?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987; 52 (4): 749 – 758.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceElizabeth W. Dunn, Lara B. Aknin, Michael Norton. Spending money on others promotes happiness. Science. 2008; 319: 1687 – 1688.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceA.M. Eikenberry. The hidden cost of cause marketing. Stanford Social Innovation Review, Summer. 2009; 51 – 55.
dc.identifier.citedreferencePam S. Ellen, Lois A. Mohr, Deborah J. Webb. Charitable programs and the retailer: Do they mix?. Journal of Retailing. 2000; 76 (3): 393 – 406.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceK. Flaherty, W. Diamond. The impact of consumers’ mental budgeting on the effectiveness of cause‐related marketing. American marketing Association Conference Proceedings. 1999; 10: 151 – 152.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceW.T. Harbaugh, U. Mayr, D.R. Harbaugh. Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science. 2007; 316 (5831): 1622 – 1625, 15 (June).
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJoseph Henrich, Robert Boyd, Samuel Bowles, Colin Camerer, Ernst Fehr, Herbert Gintis, Richard McElreath. In search of homo economicus: Behavioral experiments in 15 small‐scale societies. The American Economic Review. 2001; 91 (2): 73 – 78.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKaren Jenni, George Loewenstein. Explaining the identifiable victim effect. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. 1997; 14 (3): 235 – 257.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceU. Khan, R. Dhar. Licensing effect in consumer choice. Journal of Marketing Research. 2006; 43 (2): 259 – 266.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceS. King. Pink Ribbons Inc, University of Minnesota Press. 2006.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHarald Kunemund, Martin Rein. There is more to receiving than needing: Theoretical arguments and empirical explorations of crowding in and crowding out. Ageing and Society. 1999; 19: 93 – 121.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceD.R. Lichtenstein, M.N. Drumwright, B.M. Braig. The effect of corporate social responsibility on customer donations to corporate‐supported nonprofits. Journal of Marketing. 2004; 68 (4): 16 – 32.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceN. Mazar, C. Zhong. Do green products make us better people?. Psychological Science. 2010; 21 (4): 494 – 498.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceG. Douglas Olsen, John W. Pracejus, Norman R. Brown. When profit equals price: Consumer confusion about donation amounts in cause‐related marketing. The Journal of Public Policy and Marketing. 2003; 22 (2): 170 – 180.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMatthew Rabin. Incorporation fairness into game theory and economics. The American Economic Review. 1993; 83 (5): 1281 – 1302.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJ. Philippe Rushton, Rolald D. Chrisjohn, G. Cynthia Fekken. The altruistic personality and the self‐report altruism scale. Personality and Individual Differences. 1981; 2: 293 – 302.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceS. Sachdeva, R. Iliev, D.L. Medin. Sinning saints and saintly sinners: The paradox of moral self‐regulation. Psychological Science. 2009; 20: 523 – 528.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceShankar Sen, C.B. Bhattacharya. Does doing good always lead to doing better? Consumer reactions to corporate social responsibility. Journal of Marketing Research. 2001; 38 (2): 225 – 243.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDeborah A. Small, George Loewenstein, Paul Slovic. Sympathy and callousness: The impact of deliberative thought on donations to identifiable and statistical victims. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 2007; 102 (2): 143 – 153.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMichal Strahilevitz. The effects of product type and donation magnitude on willingness to pay more for a charity‐linked brand. Journal of Consumer Psychology. 1999; 8 (3): 215 – 241.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJames Andreoni. Cooperation in public‐goods experiments: Kindness or confusion?. The American Economic Review. 1995; 85 (4): 891 – 904.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJames Andreoni, John H. Miller. Giving According to GARP: An experimental test of the consistency of preferences for altruism. Econometrica. 2002; 70 (2): 737 – 753.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMichael J. Barone, A.T. Norman, Anthony D. Miyazaki. Consumer response to retailer use of cause related marketing: is more fit better?. Journal of Retailing, Winter. 2007; 437 – 445.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceC.D. Batson, L.L. Shaw. Evidence for altruism: Toward a pluralism of pro‐social motives. Psychological Inquiry. 1991; 2 (2): 107 – 122.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBoston Globe, October 4, 2009, “Sick of Pink”, by Kris Frieswick.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNancy R. Buchan, Rachel T.A. Croson, Robyn M. Dawes. Swift neighbors and persistent strangers: A cross‐cultural investigation of trust and reciprocity in social exchange. American Journal of Sociology. 2002; 108 (1): 168 – 206.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJ.P. Carpenter. The demand for punishment. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization. 2007; 62 (4): 522 – 542.
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.