Show simple item record

Children Seek Historical Traces of Owned Objects

dc.contributor.authorGelman, Susan A.
dc.contributor.authorManczak, Erika M.
dc.contributor.authorWas, Alexandra M.
dc.contributor.authorNoles, Nicholaus S.
dc.date.accessioned2018-02-05T16:36:06Z
dc.date.available2018-02-05T16:36:06Z
dc.date.issued2016-01
dc.identifier.citationGelman, Susan A.; Manczak, Erika M.; Was, Alexandra M.; Noles, Nicholaus S. (2016). "Children Seek Historical Traces of Owned Objects." Child Development 87(1): 239-255.
dc.identifier.issn0009-3920
dc.identifier.issn1467-8624
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/141553
dc.publisherMIT Press
dc.publisherWiley Periodicals, Inc.
dc.titleChildren Seek Historical Traces of Owned Objects
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollow
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPsychology
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciences
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Reviewed
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttps://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/141553/1/cdev12453.pdf
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttps://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/141553/2/cdev12453_am.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/cdev.12453
dc.identifier.sourceChild Development
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRakison, D. H., & Oakes, L. M. ( 2003 ). Early category and concept development: Making sense of the blooming, buzzing confusion. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFrazier, B. N., Gelman, S. A., Wilson, A., & Hood, B. ( 2009 ). Picasso paintings, moon rocks, and hand‐written Beatles lyrics: Adults’ evaluations of authentic objects. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 9, 1 – 14. doi: 10.1163/156853709X414601
dc.identifier.citedreferencePiaget, J. ( 1970 ). Genetic epistemology. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNewman, G. E., & Bloom, P. ( 2014 ). Physical contact influences how much people pay at celebrity auctions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 3705 – 3708. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1313637111
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNewman, G. E., Diesendruck, G., & Bloom, P. ( 2011 ). Celebrity contagion and the value of objects. Journal of Consumer Research, 38, 215 – 228. doi: 10.1086/658999
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFrazier, B. N., & Gelman, S. A. ( 2009 ). Developmental changes in judgments of authentic objects. Cognitive Development, 24, 284 – 292. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.06.003
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRips, L. J. ( 2011 ). Lines of thought: Central concepts in cognitive psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRosengren, K. S., Gelman, S. A., Kalish, C. W., & McCormick, M. ( 1991 ). As time goes by: Children’s early understanding of growth in animals. Child Development, 62, 1302 – 1320. doi: 10.2307/1130808
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchulz, L. E., Standing, H. R., & Bonawitz, E. B. ( 2008 ). Word, thought, and deed: The role of object categories in children’s inductive inferences and exploratory play. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1266 – 1276. doi: 10.1037/0012‐1649.44.5.1266
dc.identifier.citedreferenceScott, R. M., & Baillargeon, R. ( 2013 ). Do infants really expect agents to act efficiently? A critical test of the rationality principle. Psychological Science, 24, 466 – 474. doi: 10.1177/0956797612457395
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSetoh, P., Wu, D., Baillargeon, R., & Gelman, R. ( 2013 ). Young infants have biological expectations about animals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 15937 – 15942. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1314075110
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSloutsky, V. M., Kloos, H., & Fisher, A. V. ( 2007 ). When looks are everything: Appearance similarity versus kind information in early induction. Psychological Science, 18, 179 – 185. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‐9280.2007.01869.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSobel, D. M., Yoachim, C. M., Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., & Blumenthal, E. J. ( 2007 ). The blicket within: Preschoolers’ inferences about insides and causes. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8, 159 – 182. doi: 10.1080/15248370701202356
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSorrentino, C. M. ( 2001 ). Children and adults represent proper names as referring to unique individuals. Developmental Science, 4, 399 – 407. doi: 10.1111/1467‐7687.00181
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWalker, C. M., Lombrozo, T., Legare, C. H., & Gopnik, A. ( 2014 ). Explaining prompts children to privilege inductively rich properties. Cognition, 133, 343 – 357. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.07.008
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWare, E. A., & Booth, A. E. ( 2010 ). Form follows function: Learning about function helps children learn about shape. Cognitive Development, 25, 124 – 137. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.10.003
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWaxman, S. R., & Gelman, S. A. ( 2009 ). Early word‐learning entails reference, not merely associations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 258 – 263. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.03.006
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. ( 1988 ). Children’s understanding of the nonobvious. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 4, pp. 99 – 135 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWhite, P. A. ( 2009 ). Property transmission: An explanatory account of the role of similarity information in causal inference. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 774 – 793. doi: 10.1037/a0016970
dc.identifier.citedreferenceZelizer, V. A. R. ( 1997 ). The social meaning of money. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBadger, J. R., & Shapiro, L. R. ( 2012 ). Evidence of a transition from perceptual to category induction in 3‐ to 9‐year‐old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 131 – 146. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.03.004
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBeggan, J. K. ( 1992 ). On the social nature of nonsocial perception: The mere ownership effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 229 – 237. doi: 10.1037/0022‐3514.62.2.229
dc.identifier.citedreferenceButler, L. P., & Markman, E. M. ( 2014 ). Preschoolers use pedagogical cues to guide radical reorganization of category knowledge. Cognition, 130, 116 – 127. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.10.002
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCoventry, K. R., Griffiths, D., & Hamilton, C. J. ( 2014 ). Spatial demonstratives and perceptual space: Describing and remembering object location. Cognitive Psychology, 69, 46 – 70. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.12.001
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCsibra, G., Gergely, G., Bíró, S., Koós, O., & Brockbank, M. ( 1999 ). Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of “pure reason” in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237 – 267. doi: 10.1016/S0010‐0277(99)00039‐6
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCunningham, S. J., Vergunst, F., Macrae, C. N., & Turk, D. J. ( 2013 ). Exploring early self‐referential memory effects through ownership. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31, 289 – 301. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12005
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDeák, G. O., Ray, S. D., & Brenneman, K. ( 2003 ). Children’s perseverative appearance‐reality errors are related to emerging language skills. Child Development, 74, 944 – 964. doi: 10.1111/1467‐8624.00578
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDeJesus, J. M., Shutts, K., & Kinzler, K. D. ( 2015 ). Eww she sneezed! Contamination context affects children’s food preferences and consumption. Appetite, 87, 303 – 309. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.12.222
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFisher, A. V. ( 2011 ). Processing of perceptual information is more robust than processing of conceptual information in preschool‐age children: Evidence from costs of switching. Cognition, 119, 253 – 264. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.015
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFlavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., & Green, F. L. ( 1983 ). Development of the appearance–reality distinction. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 95 – 120. doi: 10.1016/0010‐0285(83)90005‐1
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFriedman, O., & Neary, K. R. ( 2008 ). Determining who owns what: Do children infer ownership from first possession? Cognition, 107, 829 – 849. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.002
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFriedman, O., Van de Vondervoort, J. W., Defeyter, M. A., & Neary, K. R. ( 2013 ). First possession, history, and young children’s ownership judgments. Child Development, 84, 1519 – 1525. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12080
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFriedman, W. J. ( 2003 ). Arrows of time in early childhood. Child Development, 74, 155 – 167. doi: 10.1111/1467‐8624.00527
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGanea, P. A., Shutts, K., Spelke, E. S., & DeLoache, J. S. ( 2007 ). Thinking of things unseen: Infants’ use of language to update mental representations. Psychological Science, 18, 734 – 739. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‐9280.2007.01968.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, R., Bullock, M., & Meck, E. ( 1980 ). Preschoolers’ understanding of simple object transformations. Child Development, 51, 691 – 699. doi: 10.2307/1129454
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., Frazier, B. N., Noles, N. S., Manczak, E. M., & Stilwell, S. M. ( 2015 ). How much are Harry Potter’s glasses worth? Children’s monetary evaluation of authentic objects. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16, 97 – 117. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2013.815623
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., Manczak, E. M., & Noles, N. S. ( 2012 ). The nonobvious basis of ownership: Preschool children trace the history and value of owned objects. Child Development, 83, 1732 – 1747. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‐8624.2012.01806.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., Noles, N. S., & Stilwell, S. ( 2014 ). Tracking the actions and possessions of agents. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6, 599 – 614. doi: 10.1111/tops.12106
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGutheil, G., Gelman, S. A., Klein, E., Michos, K., & Kelaita, K. ( 2008 ). Preschoolers’ use of spatiotemporal history, appearance, and proper name in determining individual identity. Cognition, 107, 366 – 380. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.014
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHall, D. G. ( 1996 ). Preschoolers’ default assumptions about word meaning: Proper names designate unique individuals. Developmental Psychology, 32, 177 – 186. doi: 10.1037/0012‐1649.32.1.177
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHejmadi, A., Rozin, P., & Siegal, M. ( 2004 ). Once in contact, always in contact: Contagious essence and conceptions of purification in American and Hindu Indian children. Developmental Psychology, 40, 467 – 476. doi: 10.1037/0012‐1649.40.4.467
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHood, B. M., & Bloom, P. ( 2008 ). Children prefer certain individuals over perfect duplicates. Cognition, 106, 455 – 462. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.01.012
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJackendoff, R. S. ( 1992 ). Languages of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. H. ( 1990 ). Experimental tests of the endowment effect and the Coase Theorem. Journal of Political Economy, 98, 1325 – 1348. doi: 10.1086/261737
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKeil, F. C. ( 2006 ). Explanation and understanding. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 227. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190100
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKelemen, D., Widdowson, D., Posner, T., Brown, A. L., & Casler, K. ( 2003 ). Teleo‐functional constraints on preschool children’s reasoning about living things. Developmental Science, 6, 329 – 345. doi: 10.1111/1467‐7687.00288
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKemler Nelson, D. G., Frankenfield, A., Morris, C., & Blair, E. ( 2000 ). Young children’s use of functional information to categorize artifacts: Three factors that matter. Cognition, 77, 133 – 168. doi: 10.1016/S0010‐0277(00)00097‐4
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLandau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S. ( 1988 ). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3, 299 – 321. doi: 10.1016/0885‐2014(88)90014‐7
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLegare, C. H., Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. ( 2009 ). Evidence for an explanation advantage in naive biological reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 177 – 194. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.06.002
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLeyton, M. ( 1992 ). Symmetry, causality, mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMeyer, M., Leslie, S., Gelman, S. A., & Stilwell, S. M. ( 2013 ). Essentialist beliefs about bodily transplants in the United States and India. Cognitive Science, 37, 668 – 710. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12023
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNancekivell, S. E., & Friedman, O. ( 2014 ). Mine, yours, no one’s: Children’s understanding of how ownership affects object use. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1845 – 1853. doi: 10.1037/a0036971
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNemeroff, C., & Rozin, P. ( 1994 ). The contagion concept in adult thinking in the United States: Transmission of germs and of interpersonal influence. Ethos, 22, 158 – 186. doi: 10.1525/eth.1994.22.2.02a00020
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNewman, G. E., & Bloom, P. ( 2012 ). Art and authenticity: The importance of originals in judgments of value. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 558 – 569. doi: 10.1037/a0026035
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.