Show simple item record

Individual differences in the effectiveness of self‐distancing for young children’s emotion regulation

dc.contributor.authorGrenell, Amanda
dc.contributor.authorPrager, Emily O.
dc.contributor.authorSchaefer, Catherine
dc.contributor.authorKross, Ethan
dc.contributor.authorDuckworth, Angela L.
dc.contributor.authorCarlson, Stephanie M.
dc.date.accessioned2019-02-12T20:24:20Z
dc.date.available2020-05-01T18:03:25Zen
dc.date.issued2019-03
dc.identifier.citationGrenell, Amanda; Prager, Emily O.; Schaefer, Catherine; Kross, Ethan; Duckworth, Angela L.; Carlson, Stephanie M. (2019). "Individual differences in the effectiveness of self‐distancing for young children’s emotion regulation." British Journal of Developmental Psychology 37(1): 84-100.
dc.identifier.issn0261-510X
dc.identifier.issn2044-835X
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/147829
dc.publisherWiley Periodicals, Inc.
dc.publisherBasic Books
dc.subject.othercognitive development
dc.subject.otherself‐regulation
dc.subject.otherpsychological distancing
dc.subject.otherindividual differences
dc.subject.otherearly childhood
dc.titleIndividual differences in the effectiveness of self‐distancing for young children’s emotion regulation
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollow
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPsychology
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelHealth Sciences
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Reviewed
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttps://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/147829/1/bjdp12259.pdf
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttps://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/147829/2/bjdp12259_am.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/bjdp.12259
dc.identifier.sourceBritish Journal of Developmental Psychology
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMoser, J. S., Dougherty, A., Mattson, W. I., Katz, B., Moran, T. P., Guevarra, D., … Kross, E. ( 2017 ). Third‐person self‐talk facilitates emotion regulation without engaging cognitive control: Converging evidence from ERP and fMRI. Scientific Reports, 7, 1 – 9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-04047-3
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKross, E., & Ayduk, O. ( 2009 ). Boundary conditions and buffering effects: Does depressive symptomology moderate the effectiveness of self‐distancing for facilitating adaptive emotional analysis? Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 923 – 927. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.04.004
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKross, E., & Ayduk, O. ( 2011 ). Making meaning out of negative experiences by self‐distancing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 187 – 191. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411408883
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKross, E., & Ayduk, O. ( 2017 ). Self‐distancing: Theory, research, and current directions. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 81 – 136. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2016.10.002
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKross, E., Ayduk, O., & Mischel, W. ( 2005 ). When asking “why” does not hurt: Distinguishing rumination from reflective processing of negative emotions. Psychological Science, 16, 709 – 715. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01600.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKross, E., Bruehlman‐Senecal, E., Park, J., Burson, A., Dougherty, A., Shablack, H., … Ayduk, O. ( 2014 ). Self‐talk as a regulatory mechanism: How you do it matters. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 304 – 324. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035173
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKross, E., Duckworth, A., Ayduk, O., Tsukayama, E., & Mischel, W. ( 2011 ). The effect of self‐distancing on adaptive versus maladaptive self‐reflection in children. Emotion, 11, 1032 – 1039. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021787
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKross, E., Gard, D., Deldin, P., Clifton, J., & Ayduk, O. ( 2012 ). “Asking why” from a distance: Its cognitive and emotional consequences for people with major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 559 – 569. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028808
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKross, E., Vickers, B. D., Orvell, A., Gainsburg, I., Moran, T. P., Boyer, M., … Ayduk, O. ( 2017 ). Third‐person self‐talk reduces Ebola worry and risk perception by enhancing rational thinking. Applied Psychology: Health and Well‐Being, 9, 387 – 409. https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12103
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLiberman, N., & Trope, Y. ( 2014 ). Traversing psychological distance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 364 – 369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.001
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMischel, W., & Baker, N. ( 1975 ). Cognitive appraisals and transformations in delay behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 254 – 261. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076272
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMoffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., … Caspi, A. ( 2011 ). A gradient of childhood self‐control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 2693 – 2698. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010076108
dc.identifier.citedreferencePenner, L. A., Guevarra, D. A., Harper, F. W., Taub, J., Phipps, S., Albrecht, T. L., & Kross, E. ( 2016 ). Self‐distancing buffers high trait anxious pediatric cancer caregivers against short‐and longer‐term distress. Clinical Psychological Science, 4, 629 – 640. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702615602864
dc.identifier.citedreferencePfeiler, T. M., Wenzel, M., Weber, H., & Kubiak, T. ( 2017 ). Adaptive modes of rumination: The role of subjective anger. Cognition and Emotion, 31, 580 – 589. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1117961
dc.identifier.citedreferencePrencipe, A., & Zelazo, P. D. ( 2005 ). Development of affective decision making for self and other: Evidence for the integration of first‐and third‐person perspectives. Psychological Science, 16, 501 – 505. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01564.x
dc.identifier.citedreferencePutnam, S. P., & Rothbart, M. K. ( 2006 ). Development of short and very short forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87, 102 – 112. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_09
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. ( 2006 ). Temperament. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3, Social, emotional, and personality development ( 6th ed., pp. 99 – 166 ). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSigel, I. ( 1970 ). The distancing hypothesis: A causal hypothesis for the acquisition of representational thought. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Early schooling: The national debate (pp. 129 – 150 ). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSinger, D. G., & Singer, J. L. ( 1990 ). The house of make‐believe: Children’s play and the developing imagination. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. ( 2001 ). Meta‐analysis of theory‐of‐mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655 – 684. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00304
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWhite, R. E., & Carlson, S. M. ( 2015 ). What would Batman do? Self‐distancing improves executive function in young children. Developmental Science, 9, 419 – 426. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12314
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWhite, R. E., Kross, E., & Duckworth, A. L. ( 2015 ). Spontaneous self‐distancing and adaptive self‐reflection across adolescence. Child Development, 86, 1272 – 1281. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12370
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWhite, R. E., Prager, E. O., Schaefer, C., Kross, E., Duckworth, A. L., & Carlson, S. M. ( 2017 ). The “Batman effect”: Improving perseverance in young children. Child Development, 88, 1563 – 1571. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12695
dc.identifier.citedreferenceZelazo, P. D., Anderson, J. E., Richler, J., Wallner‐Allen, K., Beaumont, J. L., & Weintraub, S. ( 2013 ). II. NIH toolbox cognition battery (CB): Measuring executive function and attention. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 78, 16 – 33. https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12032
dc.identifier.citedreferenceZhou, Q., Chen, S. H., & Main, A. ( 2012 ). Commonalities and differences in the research on children’s effortful control and executive function: A call for an integrated model of self‐regulation. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 112 – 121. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00176.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceApperly, I. A., & Carroll, D. J. ( 2009 ). How do symbols affect 3 to 4 year olds’ executive function? Evidence from a reverse‐contingency task. Developmental Science, 12, 1070 – 1082. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00856.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBenson, J. E., Sabbagh, M. A., Carlson, S. M., & Zelazo, P. D. ( 2013 ). Individual differences in executive functioning predict preschoolers’ improvement from theory‐of‐mind training. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1615 – 1627. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031056
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBrenner, E. M., & Salovey, P. ( 1997 ). Emotion regulation during childhood: Developmental, interpersonal, and individual considerations. In P. Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development, and emotional intelligence: Educational implications (pp. 168 – 192 ). New York, NY: Basic Books.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBurrage, M. S., Ponitz, C. C., McCready, E. A., Shah, P., Sims, B. C., Jewkes, A. M., & Morrison, F. J. ( 2008 ). Age‐and schooling‐related effects on executive functions in young children: A natural experiment. Child Neuropsychology, 14, 510 – 524. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040701756917
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarlson, S. M., Davis, A. C., & Leach, J. G. ( 2005 ). Less is more: Executive function and symbolic representation in preschool children. Psychological Science, 16, 609 – 616. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01583.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. ( 2001 ). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032 – 1053. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00333
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarlson, S. M., & Wang, T. S. ( 2007 ). Inhibitory control and emotion regulation in preschool children. Cognitive Development, 22, 489 – 510. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.08.002
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarlson, S. M., Zelazo, P. D., & Faja, S. ( 2013 ). Executive function. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), Oxford handbook of developmental psychology, Vol. 1 (pp. 706 – 743 ). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCole, P. M., Dennis, T. M., Smith‐Simon, K. E., & Cohen, L. H. ( 2009 ). Preschoolers’ emotion regulation strategy understanding: Relation with emotion socialization and child self‐regulation. Social Development, 18, 324 – 352. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00503.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDavis, H. L., & Pratt, C. ( 1996 ). The development of children’s theory of mind: The working memory explanation. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 25 – 31. https://doi.org/10.1080/00049539508258765
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDiamond, A., & Ling, D. S. ( 2016 ). Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 34 – 48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.005
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDuckworth, A. L., & Carlson, S. M. ( 2013 ). Self‐regulation and school success. In B. W. Sokol, F. M. E. Grouzet & U. Müller (Eds.), Self‐regulation and autonomy: Social and developmental dimensions of human conduct (pp. 208 – 230 ). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. ( 2007 ). Peabody picture vocabulary test ( 4th ed. ). San Antonio, TX: Pearson.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEisenberg, N., & Sulik, M. J. ( 2012 ). Emotion‐related self‐regulation in children. Teaching of Psychology, 39, 77 – 83. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628311430172
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGoldsmith, H. H., Reilly, J., Lemery, K. S., Longley, S., & Prescott, A. ( 1999 ). The laboratory temperament assessment battery: Preschool version. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKarniol, R., Galili, L., Shtilerman, D., Naim, R., Stern, K., Manjoch, H., & Silverman, R. ( 2011 ). Why Superman can wait: Cognitive self‐transformation in the delay of gratification paradigm. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 40, 307 – 317. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2011.546040
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKross, E., & Ayduk, O. ( 2008 ). Facilitating adaptive emotional analysis: Distinguishing distanced‐analysis of depressive experiences from immersed‐analysis and distraction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 924 – 938. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208315938
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.