Show simple item record

The structural and motivational role of the unique lip‐flip movement in the gelada (Theropithecus gelada) facial display repertoire

dc.contributor.authorLazow, Stefanie P.
dc.contributor.authorBergman, Thore J.
dc.date.accessioned2020-06-03T15:23:31Z
dc.date.availableWITHHELD_13_MONTHS
dc.date.available2020-06-03T15:23:31Z
dc.date.issued2020-06
dc.identifier.citationLazow, Stefanie P.; Bergman, Thore J. (2020). "The structural and motivational role of the unique lip‐flip movement in the gelada (Theropithecus gelada) facial display repertoire." American Journal of Physical Anthropology 172(2): 280-290.
dc.identifier.issn0002-9483
dc.identifier.issn1096-8644
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/155514
dc.description.abstractObjectivesHuman language represents an extreme form of communicative complexity. Primate facial display complexity, which depends upon facial mobility, can be used as a model for the study of the evolution of communicative complexity. The gelada (Theropithecus gelada) is the only primate that can produce a lip‐flip eversion. This study investigates the role of the lip‐flip relative to the bared‐teeth display to understand its role in generating communicative complexity.Materials and methodsWe reviewed videos of gelada social interactions. We utilized the facial action coding system (FACS) to define structural component action units (AUs) of each display. We inferred display motivation from the behaviors of the display sender.ResultsThe lip‐flip was used only in combination with the essential AUs of the bared‐teeth display, serving as an optional structural element added to produce a structural variant. Both the bared‐teeth display with and without a lip‐flip occurred most frequently with nonaggressive, submissive behaviors. The lip‐flip was more frequently preceded by approach than the bared‐teeth display, especially in males. The lip‐flip was also present in the majority of structurally blended facial displays though the motivation of the non‐lip‐flip parent display often dominated.DiscussionThe lip‐flip may potentially function as an indicator of benign intent after an approach or as an intensifying component of nonaggressive intent. Adaptations to increase facial mobility in geladas via facilitating the lip‐flip may promote increased communicative complexity through increased conspicuousness and motivational signaling specification or intensification.
dc.publisherJohn Wiley & Sons, Inc.
dc.subject.otherfacial expression
dc.subject.otherfacial mobility
dc.subject.othergelada
dc.subject.otherbared‐teeth display
dc.subject.otherfacial display
dc.titleThe structural and motivational role of the unique lip‐flip movement in the gelada (Theropithecus gelada) facial display repertoire
dc.typeArticle
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollow
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelAnthropology
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciences
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Reviewed
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttps://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/155514/1/ajpa24031_am.pdf
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttps://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/155514/2/ajpa24031.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1002/ajpa.24031
dc.identifier.sourceAmerican Journal of Physical Anthropology
dc.identifier.citedreferencePartan, S. R., & Marler, P. ( 1999 ). Communication goes multimodal. Science, 283, 1272 – 1273. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.283.5406.1272
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMicheletta, J., Engelhardt, A., Matthews, L., Agil, M., & Waller, B. ( 2013 ). Multicomponent and multimodal lipsmacking in crested macaques ( Macaca nigra ). American Journal of Primatology, 75 ( 7 ), 763 – 773. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22105
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMori, U. ( 1979a ). Individual relationships within a unit. In M. Kawai (Ed.), Ecological and sociological studies of gelada baboons: Contributions to primatology (Vol. 16, pp. 93 – 124 ). Tokyo: Kodansha Ltd..
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMori, U. ( 1979b ). Reproductive behavior. In M. Kawai (Ed.), Ecological and sociological studies of gelada baboons: Contributions to primatology (Vol. 16, pp. 183 – 199 ). Tokyo: Kodansha Ltd..
dc.identifier.citedreferenceParr, L. A., Cohen, M., & de Waal, F. ( 2005 ). Influence of social context on the use of blended and graded facial displays in chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology, 26, 73 – 103. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-005-0724-z
dc.identifier.citedreferenceParr, L. A., Waller, B. M., Burrows, A. M., Gothard, K. M., & Vick, S. J. ( 2010 ). Brief communication: MaqFACS: A muscle‐based facial movement coding system for the rhesus macaque. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 143, 625 – 630. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.21401
dc.identifier.citedreferenceParr, L. A., Waller, B. M., & Micheletta, J. ( 2016 ). Nonverbal communication in primates: Observational and experimental approaches. In D. Matsumoto, H. C. Hwang, & M. G. Frank (Eds.), APA handbook of nonverbal communication. Washington, D.C.:American Psychological Association.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceParr, L., & Waller, B. M. ( 2006 ). Understanding chimpanzee facial expression: Insights into the evolution of communication. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 221 – 228. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl031
dc.identifier.citedreferenceParr, L. A., Waller, B. M., Vick, S. J., & Bard, K. A. ( 2007 ). Classifying chimpanzee facial expressions using muscle action. Emotion, 7, 172 – 181. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.172
dc.identifier.citedreferencePartan, S. R., & Marler, P. ( 2005 ). Issues in the classification of multimodal communication signals. The American Naturalist, 166, 231 – 245. https://doi.org/10.1086/431246
dc.identifier.citedreferenceScheider, L., Liebal, K., Ona, L., Burrows, A. M., & Waller, B. M. ( 2014 ). A comparison of facial expression properties in five hylobatid species. American Journal of Primatology, 76 ( 7 ), 618 – 628. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22255
dc.identifier.citedreferenceShannon, C. E. ( 1948 ). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Labs Technical Journal, 27, 379 – 423. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb01338.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSlocombe, K. E., Waller, B. M., & Liebal, K. ( 2011 ). The language void: The need for multimodality in primate communication research. Animal Behaviour, 81 ( 5 ), 919 – 924. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.02.002
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSnyder‐Mackler, N., Beehner, J. C., & Bergman, T. J. ( 2012 ). Defining higher levels in the multilevel societies of geladas ( Theropithecus gelada). International Journal of Primatology, 33, 1054 – 1068.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceThierry, B., Demaria, C., Preuschoft, S., & Desportes, C. ( 1989 ). Structural convergence between silent bared‐teeth display and relaxed open‐mouth display in the Tonkean macaque ( Macaca tonkeana ). Folia Primatol (Basel), 52 ( 3–4 ), 178 – 184. https://doi.org/10.1159/000156396
dc.identifier.citedreferencevan Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. ( 1967 ). The facial displays of catarrhine monkeys and apes. In D. Morris (Ed.), Primate ethology (pp. 7 – 68 ). Chicago, IL: Aldine Publishing Company.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceViera, A. J., & Garrett, J. M. ( 2005 ). Understanding interobserver agreement: The kappa statistic. Family Medicine, 37 ( 5 ), 360 – 363 PMID: 15883903.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWaller, B., Lembeck, M., Kuchenbuch, P., Burrows, A., & Liebal, K. ( 2012 ). GibbonFACS: A muscle‐based facial movement coding system for hylobatids. Internal Journal of Primatology, 33 ( 4 ), 809 – 821. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-012-9611-6
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWaller, B. M., & Michelatta, J. ( 2013 ). Facial expression in nonhuman animals. Emotion Review, 5 ( 1 ), 54 – 59. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912451503
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWaller, B. M., Warmelink, L., Liebal, K., Michelatta, J., & Sclocombe, K. E. ( 2013 ). Pseudoreplication: A widespread problem in primate communication research. Animal Behaviour, 86 ( 2 ), 483 – 488.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWaller, B. M., Whitehouse, J., & Micheletta, J. ( 2016 ). Macaques can predict social outcomes from facial expressions. Animal Cognition, 19 ( 5 ), 1031 – 1036. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-0992-3
dc.identifier.citedreferenceZimmerman, E. ( 2017 ). Evolutionary origins of primate vocal communication: Diversity, flexibility, and complexity of vocalizations in basal primates. In R. M. Quam, M. A. Ramsier, R. R. Fay, & A. N. Popper (Eds.), Primate hearing and communication (pp. 109 – 140 ). Berlin, Germany: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-59478-1_5
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAlvarez, F., & Consul, C. ( 1978 ). The structure of social behavior in Theropithecus gelada. Primates, 19, 45 – 59. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02373226
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBout, N., & Thierry, B. ( 2005 ). Peaceful meaning for the silent bared‐teeth displays of mandrills. International Journal of Primatology, 26 ( 6 ), 1215 – 1228. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-005-8850-1
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBramblett, C. A. ( 1970 ). Coalitions among gelada baboons. Primates, 11, 327 – 333. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01730637
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBraune, P., Schmidt, S., & Zimmerman, E. ( 2008 ). Acoustic divergence in the communication of cryptic species of nocturnal primates. BMC Biology, 6, 19. https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7007-6-19
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBurrows, A. M. ( 2008 ). The facial expression musculature in primates and its evolutionary significance. BioEssays, 30, 212 – 225. https://doi.org/10.1002/bies.20719
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCaeiro, C., Waller, B., Zimmerman, E., Burrows, A., & Davila‐Ross, M. ( 2013 ). OrangFACS: A muscle‐based facial movement coding system for orangutans (pongo spp.). International Journal of Primatology, 34 ( 1 ), 115 – 129. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-012-9652-x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCorballis, M. C. ( 2010 ). Mirror neurons and the evolution of language. Brain & Language, 112, 25 – 35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2009.02.002
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDawkins, M. S., & Guilford, T. ( 1991 ). The corruption of honest signaling. Animal Behaviour, 41, 865 – 873. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80353-7
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDobson, S. D. ( 2006 ). Comparative study of facial mobility in anthropoid primates (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). St. Louis, MO: Washington University in St. Louis.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDobson, S. D. ( 2009a ). Allometry of facial mobility in anthropoid primates: Implications for the evolution of facial expression. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 138, 70 – 81. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.20902
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDobson, S. D. ( 2009b ). Socioecological correlates of facial mobility in nonhuman anthropoids. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 139, 413 – 420. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.21007
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDunbar, R. I. M., & Dunbar, P. ( 1975 ). Social dynamics of gelada baboons. In Contributions to primatology (Vol. 6 ). Basel, NY: Karger.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEkman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Hager, J. C. ( 2002 ). Facial action coding system: The manual on CD‐ROM. Salt Lake City, UT: Research Nexus.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFedigan, L. M. ( 1972 ). Roles and activities of male geladas ( Theropithecus gelada ). Behaviour, 41, 82 – 90. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853972X00211
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFreeberg, T. M. ( 2006 ). Social complexity can drive vocal complexity: Group size influences vocal information in Carolina chickadees. Psychological Science, 17, 557 – 561. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01743.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFreeberg, T. M., Dunbar, R. I. M., & Ord, T. J. ( 2012 ). Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 1785 – 1801. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0213
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGamba, M., Friard, O., & Giacoma, C. ( 2012 ). Vocal tract morphology determines species‐specific features in vocal signals of lemurs (Eulemur). International Journal of Primatology, 33 ( 6 ), 1453 – 1466. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-012-9635-y
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGentilucci, M., & Corballis, M. C. ( 2006 ). From manual gesture to speech: A gradual transition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30 ( 7 ), 949 – 960. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuobiorev.2006.02.004
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGhazanfar, A. A., & Takahashi, D. Y. ( 2014 ). Facial expressions and the evolution of the speech rhythm. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26 ( 6 ), 1196 – 1207. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00575
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGustison, M. L., & Bergman, T. J. ( 2016 ). Vocal complexity influences females responses to gelada male calls. Scientific Reports, 6, 19680. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep19680
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHill, W. C. O. ( 1969 ). On the muscles of expression in the gelada Theropithecus gelada (Rüppell) (primates, Cercopithecidae). Zoomorphology Tiere, 65, 274 – 286.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKawai, M. ( 1979 ). Auditory communication and social relations. In M. Kawai (Ed.), Ecological and sociological studies of gelada baboons: Contributions to primatology (Vol. 16, pp. 219 – 242 ). Tokyo: Kodansha Ltd..
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKawai, M., Ohsawa, H., Mori, U., & Dunbar, R. ( 1983 ). Social organization of gelada baboons: Social units and definitions. Primates, 24, 13 – 24.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKean, D., Tiddi, B., Fahy, M., Heistermann, M., Schino, G., & Wheller, B. C. ( 2017 ). Feeling anxious? The mechanisms of vocal deception in tufted capuchin monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 130, 37 – 46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.06.008
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKrebs, J. R., & Dawkins, R. ( 1984 ). Animal signals: Reading and manipulation. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach ( 2nd ed., pp. 380 – 402 ). Oxford: Blackwell.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKroodsma, D. E. ( 1977 ). Correlates of song organization among North American wrens. The American Naturalist, 111 ( 9831 ), 995 – 1008 DOI.org/10.1086/283228.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLeone, A., Ferrari, P. F., & Palagi, E. ( 2014 ). Different yawns, different functions? Testing social hypotheses on spontaneous yawning in Theropithecus gelada. Scientific Reports, 4, 4010. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep04010
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMarler, P. ( 1965 ). Communication in monkeys and apes. In I. DeVore (Ed.), Primate behavior: Field studies of monkeys and apes (pp. 544 – 584 ). New York, NY: Holt, Rinechart, and Winston.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMaynard Smith, J., & Harper, D. ( 2003 ). Animal signals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMcComb, K., & Semple, S. ( 2005 ). Coevolution of vocal communication and sociality in primates. Biology Letters, 1, 381 – 385. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2005.0366
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.