The evolutionary origins of natural pedagogy: Rhesus monkeys show sustained attention following nonsocial cues versus social communicative signals
dc.contributor.author | Bettle, Rosemary | |
dc.contributor.author | Rosati, Alexandra G. | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2021-01-05T18:49:13Z | |
dc.date.available | WITHHELD_13_MONTHS | |
dc.date.available | 2021-01-05T18:49:13Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2021-01 | |
dc.identifier.citation | Bettle, Rosemary; Rosati, Alexandra G. (2021). "The evolutionary origins of natural pedagogy: Rhesus monkeys show sustained attention following nonsocial cues versus social communicative signals." Developmental Science 24(1): n/a-n/a. | |
dc.identifier.issn | 1363-755X | |
dc.identifier.issn | 1467-7687 | |
dc.identifier.uri | https://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/163960 | |
dc.description.abstract | The natural pedagogy hypothesis proposes that human infants preferentially attend to communicative signals from others, facilitating rapid cultural learning. In this view, sensitivity to such signals is a uniquely human adaptation and as such nonhuman animals should not produce or utilize these communicative signals. We test these evolutionary predictions by examining sensitivity to communicative cues in 206 rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) using an expectancy looking time task modeled on prior work with infants. Monkeys observed a human actor who either made eye contact and vocalized to the monkey (social cue), or waved a fruit in front of her face and produced a tapping sound (nonsocial cue). The actor then either looked at an object (referential look) or looked toward empty space (look away). We found that, unlike human infants in analogous situations, rhesus monkeys looked longer at events following nonsocial cues, regardless of the demonstrator’s subsequent looking behavior. Moreover younger and older monkeys showed similar patterns of responses across development. These results provide support for the natural pedagogy hypothesis, while also highlighting evolutionary changes in human sensitivity to communicative signals.This study examines how rhesus monkeys respond to social communicative cues, which are proposed to underpin human‐unique forms of cultural learning. In an expectancy looking time task modeled on prior infant studies, we found that monkeys exhibited longer looking responses following nonsocial attention‐grabbing cues compared to social cues, and did not expect social cues to be followed by referential actions, unlike human infants. These results provide support for the proposal that sensitivity to ‘ostensive’ cues may be a human cognitive adaptation. | |
dc.publisher | Classic readings in evolutionary psychopathology | |
dc.publisher | Wiley Periodicals, Inc. | |
dc.subject.other | pedagogy | |
dc.subject.other | primates | |
dc.subject.other | social cognition | |
dc.subject.other | gaze | |
dc.subject.other | comparative development | |
dc.title | The evolutionary origins of natural pedagogy: Rhesus monkeys show sustained attention following nonsocial cues versus social communicative signals | |
dc.type | Article | |
dc.rights.robots | IndexNoFollow | |
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevel | Pediatrics | |
dc.subject.hlbtoplevel | Health Sciences | |
dc.description.peerreviewed | Peer Reviewed | |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/163960/1/desc12987.pdf | |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/163960/2/desc12987_am.pdf | |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1111/desc.12987 | |
dc.identifier.source | Developmental Science | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Schmidt, M. F., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. ( 2011 ). Young children attribute normativity to novel actions without pedagogy or normative language. Developmental Science, 14 ( 3 ), 530 – 539. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐7687.2010.01000.x | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rosati, A. G., Wobber, V., Hughes, K., & Santos, L. R. ( 2014 ). Comparative developmental psychology: How is human cognitive development unique? Evolutionary Psychology, 12 ( 2 ), 147470491401200211. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491401200211 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Santos, L. R., Nissen, A. G., & Ferrugia, J. A. ( 2006 ). Rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta, know what others can and cannot hear. Animal Behaviour, 71 ( 5 ), 1175 – 1181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.10.007 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Senju, A., & Csibra, G. ( 2008 ). Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Current Biology, 18 ( 9 ), 668 – 671. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.03.059 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Simpson, E. A., Miller, G. M., Ferrari, P. F., Suomi, S. J., & Paukner, A. ( 2016 ). Neonatal imitation and early social experience predict gaze following abilities in infant monkeys. Scientific Reports, 6, 20233. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep20233 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Simpson, E. A., Nicolini, Y., Shetler, M., Suomi, S. J., Ferrari, P. F., & Paukner, A. ( 2016 ). Experience‐independent sex differences in newborn macaques: Females are more social than males. Scientific Reports, 6, 19669. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep19669 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Simpson, E. A., Paukner, A., Sclafani, V., Suomi, S. J., & Ferrari, P. F. ( 2013 ). Lipsmacking imitation skill in newborn macaques is predictive of social partner discrimination. PLoS One, 8 ( 12 ), e82921. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0082921 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Spelke, E., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., & Jacobson, K. ( 1992 ). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605 – 632. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033‐295X.99.4.605 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. ( 1986 ). Relevance: Communication and cognition (Vol. 142 ). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Téglás, E., Gergely, A., Kupán, K., Miklósi, Á., & Topál, J. ( 2012 ). Dogs’ gaze following is tuned to human communicative signals. Current Biology, 22 ( 3 ), 209 – 212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.018 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. ( 2009 ). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364 ( 1528 ), 2405 – 2415. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Teufel, C., Gutmann, A., Pirow, R., & Fischer, J. ( 2010 ). Facial expressions modulate the ontogenetic trajectory of gaze‐following among monkeys. Developmental Science, 13 ( 6 ), 913 – 922. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐7687.2010.00956.x | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Thierry, B. ( 2000 ). Management patterns across macaque species. Natural Conflict Resolution, 106 – 128. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Thornton, A., & McAuliffe, K. ( 2006 ). Teaching in wild meerkats. Science, 313 ( 5784 ), 227 – 229. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Thornton, A., & Raihani, N. J. ( 2008 ). The evolution of teaching. Animal Behaviour, 75 ( 6 ), 1823 – 1836. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.12.014 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Tomasello, M. ( 2009 ). The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Tomasello, M., & Call, J. ( 2019 ). Thirty years of great ape gestures. Animal Cognition, 22 ( 4 ), 461 – 469. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071‐018‐1167‐1 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Tomasello, M., Hare, B., & Fogleman, T. ( 2001 ). The ontogeny of gaze following in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, and rhesus macaques, Macaca Mulatta. Animal Behaviour, 61 ( 2 ), 335 – 343. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1598 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Topál, J., Gergely, G., Erdőhegyi, Á., Csibra, G., & Miklósi, Á. ( 2009 ). Differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, wolves, and human infants. Science, 325 ( 5945 ), 1269 – 1272. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Topál, J., Gergely, G., Miklósi, Á., Erdőhegyi, Á., & Csibra, G. ( 2008 ). Infants’ perseverative search errors are induced by pragmatic misinterpretation. Science, 321 ( 5897 ), 1831 – 1834. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wu, R., & Kirkham, N. Z. ( 2010 ). No two cues are alike: Depth of learning during infancy is dependent on what orients attention. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107 ( 2 ), 118 – 136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.014 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Yoon, J. M., Johnson, M. H., & Csibra, G. ( 2008 ). Communication‐induced memory biases in preverbal infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105 ( 36 ), 13690 – 13695. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0804388105 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Almeling, L., Hammerschmidt, K., Sennhenn‐Reulen, H., Freund, A. M., & Fischer, J. ( 2016 ). Motivational shifts in aging monkeys and the origins of social selectivity. Current Biology, 26 ( 13 ), 1744 – 1749. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.066 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Baldwin, D. A. ( 1993 ). Early referential understanding: Infants’ ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology, 29 ( 5 ), 832. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012‐1649.29.5.832 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Baron‐Cohen, S. ( 1997 ). How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreading (pp. 207 – 239 ). The maladapted mind: Classic readings in evolutionary psychopathology. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bates D., Mächler M., Bolker B., & Walker S. ( 2015 ). Fitting linear mixed‐effects models usinglme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67 ( 1 ), 1 – 48. http://dx.doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bercovitch, F. B., Widdig, A., Trefilov, A., Kessler, M. J., Berard, J. D., Schmidtke, J., … Krawczak, M. ( 2003 ). A longitudinal study of age‐specific reproductive output and body condition among male rhesus macaques. Macaca Mulatta Naturwissenschaften, 90 ( 7 ), 309 – 312. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114‐003‐0436‐1 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bettle, R., & Rosati, A. G. ( 2019 ). Flexible gaze‐following in rhesus monkeys. Animal Cognition, 1 – 14. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071‐019‐01263‐4 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bolker, B. M., Brooks, M. E., Clark, C. J., Geange, S. W., Poulsen, J. R., Stevens, M. H. H., & White, J.‐S.‐S. ( 2009 ). Generalized linear mixed models: A practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 24 ( 3 ), 127 – 135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2008.10.008 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. ( 2006 ). Making inferences about the location of hidden food: Social dog, causal ape. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120 ( 1 ), 38. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735‐7036.120.1.38 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Byrne, R. W., Cartmill, E., Genty, E., Graham, K. E., Hobaiter, C., & Tanner, J. ( 2017 ). Great ape gestures: Intentional communication with a rich set of innate signals. Animal Cognition, 20 ( 4 ), 755 – 769. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071‐017‐1096‐4 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. ( 1990 ). Preference for infant‐directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61 ( 5 ), 1584 – 1595. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130766 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Csibra, G. ( 2010 ). Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language, 25 ( 2 ), 141 – 168. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468‐0017.2009.01384.x | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. ( 2009 ). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13 ( 4 ), 148 – 153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. ( 2011 ). Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366 ( 1567 ), 1149 – 1157. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0319 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Csibra, G., & Volein, A. ( 2008 ). Infants can infer the presence of hidden objects from referential gaze information. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26 ( 1 ), 1 – 11. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151007X185987 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | De Petrillo, F., & Rosati, A. G. ( 2019 ). Rhesus macaques use probabilities to predict future events. Evolution and Human Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.05.006 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Dettmer, A. M., Kaburu, S. S. K., Simpson, E. A., Paukner, A., Sclafani, V., Byers, K. L., … Ferrari, P. F. ( 2016 ). Neonatal face‐to‐face interactions promote later social behaviour in infant rhesus monkeys. Nature Communications, 7, 11940. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms11940 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Drayton, L. A., & Santos, L. R. ( 2017 ). Do rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta, understand what others know when gaze following? Animal Behaviour, 134, 193 – 199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.10.016 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Emery, N. J., Lorincz, E. N., Perrett, D. I., Oram, M. W., & Baker, C. I. ( 1997 ). Gaze following and joint attention in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111 ( 3 ), 286. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735‐7036.111.3.286 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. ( 2002 ). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99 ( 14 ), 9602 – 9605. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.152159999 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ferrari, P. F., Paukner, A., Ionica, C., & Suomi, S. J. ( 2009 ). Reciprocal face‐to‐face communication between rhesus macaque mothers and their newborn infants. Current Biology, 19 ( 20 ), 1768 – 1772. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.055 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fischer, J., & Price, T. ( 2017 ). Meaning, intention, and inference in primate vocal communication. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 82, 22 – 31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.014 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Flom, R., Lee, K., & Muir, D. ( 2017 ). Gaze‐following: Its development and significance. Psychology Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Flombaum, J. I., & Santos, L. R. ( 2005 ). Rhesus monkeys attribute perceptions to others. Current Biology, 15 ( 5 ), 447 – 452. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2004.12.076 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. ( 2006 ). Sylvia’s recipe: The role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge (pp. 229 – 255 ). Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition, and human interaction. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gómez, J. C. ( 2005 ). Species comparative studies and cognitive development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9 ( 3 ), 118 – 125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.004 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gouzoules, S., Gouzoules, H., & Marler, P. ( 1984 ). Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) screams: Representational signalling in the recruitment of agonistic aid. Animal Behaviour, 32 ( 1 ), 182 – 193. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003‐3472(84)80336‐X | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gredebäck, G., Astor, K., & Fawcett, C. ( 2018 ). Gaze following is not dependent on ostensive cues: A critical test of natural pedagogy. Child Development, 89 ( 6 ), 2091 – 2098. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13026 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Grice, H. P. ( 1989 ). Studies in the Way of Words. Harvard University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Grossmann, T., Johnson, M. H., Lloyd‐Fox, S., Blasi, A., Deligianni, F., Elwell, C., & Csibra, G. ( 2008 ). Early cortical specialization for face‐to‐face communication in human infants. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275 ( 1653 ), 2803 – 2811. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hare, B. ( 2007 ). From nonhuman to human mind: What changed and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16 ( 2 ), 60 – 64. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐8721.2007.00476.x | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hare, B. ( 2017 ). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 155 – 186. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., & Tomasello, M. ( 2002 ). The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science, 298 ( 5598 ), 1634 – 1636. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hare, B., Plyusnina, I., Ignacio, N., Schepina, O., Stepika, A., Wrangham, R., & Trut, L. ( 2005 ). Social cognitive evolution in captive foxes is a correlated by‐product of experimental domestication. Current Biology, 15 ( 3 ), 226 – 230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.040 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. ( 2005 ). Human‐like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9 ( 9 ), 439 – 444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., & Driver, J. ( 1998 ). Adult’s eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9 ( 2 ), 131 – 134. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467‐9280.00024 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hughes, K. D., & Santos, L. R. ( 2012 ). Rotational displacement skills in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126 ( 4 ), 421. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028757 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. ( 2005 ). Domestic goats, Capra hircus, follow gaze direction and use social cues in an object choice task. Animal Behaviour, 69 ( 1 ), 11 – 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.008 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kaminski, J., Schulz, L., & Tomasello, M. ( 2012 ). How dogs know when communication is intended for them. Developmental Science, 15 ( 2 ), 222 – 232. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐7687.2011.01120.x | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kano, F., Moore, R., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., Tomonaga, M., & Call, J. ( 2018 ). Human ostensive signals do not enhance gaze following in chimpanzees, but do enhance object‐oriented attention. Animal Cognition, 21 ( 5 ), 715 – 728. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071‐018‐1205‐z | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kaplan, H., Lancaster, J., & Robson, A. ( 2003 ). Embodied capital and the evolutionary economics of the human life span. Population and Development Review, 29, 152 – 182. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Maestripieri, D., & Wallen, K. ( 1997 ). Affiliative and submissive communication in rhesus macaques. Primates, 38 ( 2 ), 127 – 138. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02382003 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Marticorena, D. C., Ruiz, A. M., Mukerji, C., Goddu, A., & Santos, L. R. ( 2011 ). Monkeys represent others’ knowledge but not their beliefs. Developmental Science, 14 ( 6 ), 1406 – 1416. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐7687.2011.01085.x | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Martin, A., & Santos, L. R. ( 2014 ). The origins of belief representation: Monkeys fail to automatically represent others’ beliefs. Cognition, 130 ( 3 ), 300 – 308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.016 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Matsuzawa, T. ( 2007 ). Comparative cognitive development. Developmental Science, 10 ( 1 ), 97 – 103. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐7687.2007.00570.x | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Meltzoff, A. N., Kuhl, P. K., Movellan, J., & Sejnowski, T. J. ( 2009 ). Foundations for a new science of learning. Science, 325 ( 5938 ), 284 – 288. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Moore, R. ( 2016 ). Gricean communication and cognitive development. The Philosophical Quarterly, 67 ( 267 ), 303 – 326. https://doi.org/10.1093/pq/pqw049 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Morrill, R. J., Paukner, A., Ferrari, P. F., & Ghazanfar, A. A. ( 2012 ). Monkey lipsmacking develops like the human speech rhythm. Developmental Science, 15 ( 4 ), 557 – 568. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐7687.2012.01149.x | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Musgrave, S., Morgan, D., Lonsdorf, E., Mundry, R., & Sanz, C. ( 2016 ). Tool transfers are a form of teaching among chimpanzees. Scientific Reports, 6, 34783. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep34783 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Partan, S. R. ( 2002 ). Single and multichannel signal composition: Facial expressions and vocalizations of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Behaviour, 139 ( 8 ), 993 – 1028. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685390260337877 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | R Development Core Team. ( 2019 ). A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: Austria. Retrieved from http://www.R‐project.org | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rawlins, R. G., & Kessler, M. J. ( 1986 ). The history of the Cayo Santiago colony. The Cayo Santiago Macaques, 13 – 45. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rosati, A. G., Arre, A. M., Platt, M. L., & Santos, L. R. ( 2016 ). Rhesus monkeys show human‐like changes in gaze following across the lifespan. Paper presented at the Proc. R. Soc. B. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rosati, A. G., Arre, A. M., Platt, M. L., & Santos, L. R. ( 2018 ). Developmental shifts in social cognition: Socio‐emotional biases across the lifespan in rhesus monkeys. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 72 ( 10 ), 163. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265‐018‐2573‐8 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rosati, A. G., & Santos, L. R. ( 2016 ). Spontaneous metacognition in rhesus monkeys. Psychological Science, 27 ( 9 ), 1181 – 1191. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616653737 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rosati, A. G., & Santos, L. R. ( 2017 ). Tolerant Barbary macaques maintain juvenile levels of social attention in old age, but despotic rhesus macaques do not. Animal Behaviour, 130, 199 – 207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.06.019 | |
dc.owningcollname | Interdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed |
Files in this item
Remediation of Harmful Language
The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.
Accessibility
If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.