Show simple item record

How Bilingualism Informs Theory of Mind Development

dc.contributor.authorYu, Chi‐lin
dc.contributor.authorKovelman, Ioulia
dc.contributor.authorWellman, Henry M.
dc.date.accessioned2021-09-08T14:36:52Z
dc.date.available2022-10-08 10:36:51en
dc.date.available2021-09-08T14:36:52Z
dc.date.issued2021-09
dc.identifier.citationYu, Chi‐lin ; Kovelman, Ioulia; Wellman, Henry M. (2021). "How Bilingualism Informs Theory of Mind Development." Child Development Perspectives (3): 154-159.
dc.identifier.issn1750-8592
dc.identifier.issn1750-8606
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/169322
dc.description.abstractThe possibility and nature of bilingual advantage for theory of mind (ToM), that is, young bilingual children outperforming their monolingual peers, have been discussed increasingly since the first research on the topic was published in 2003. Because accumulating evidence demonstrates a ToM advantage for bilingual individuals, in this article, we focus on how this advantage arises. We consider how current theoretical positions, including executive function, metalinguistic awareness, and sociolinguistic awareness accounts, explain such an advantage in young bilingual children. These theoretical accounts receive some, but only partial, support, so further research and theory are needed to understand comprehensively the relationship between bilingualism and ToM.
dc.publisherUniversity of Toronto
dc.publisherWiley Periodicals, Inc.
dc.subject.othertheory of mind
dc.subject.otherbilingualism
dc.titleHow Bilingualism Informs Theory of Mind Development
dc.typeArticle
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollow
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPsychology
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelHealth Sciences
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciences
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Reviewed
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/169322/1/cdep12412.pdf
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/169322/2/cdep12412_am.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/cdep.12412
dc.identifier.sourceChild Development Perspectives
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMiyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. ( 2000 ). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex - frontal lobe- tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49 - 100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGenesee, F., Boivin, I., & Nicoladis, E. ( 1996 ). Talking with strangers: A study of bilingual children- s communicative competence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 427 - 442. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400008183
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGoetz, P. J. ( 2003 ). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 1 - 15. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728903001007
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGordon, K. R. ( 2016 ). High proficiency across two languages is related to better mental state reasoning for bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 43, 407 - 424. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000915000276
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGreenberg, A., Bellana, B., & Bialystok, E. ( 2013 ). Perspective- taking ability in bilingual children: Extending advantages in executive control to spatial reasoning. Cognitive Development, 28, 41 - 50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.10.002
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHan, S., & Lee, K. ( 2013 ). Cognitive and affective perspective- taking ability of young bilinguals in South Korea. Child Studies in Diverse Contexts, 3, 69 - 80. https://doi.org/10.5723/csdc.2013.3.1.069
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHartanto, A., Toh, W. X., & Yang, H. ( 2019 ). Bilingualism narrows socioeconomic disparities in executive functions and self- regulatory behaviors during early childhood: Evidence from the early childhood longitudinal study. Child Development, 90, 1215 - 1235. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13032
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKovács, à . M. ( 2009 ). Early bilingualism enhances mechanisms of false- belief reasoning. Developmental Science, 12, 48 - 54. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- 7687.2008.00742.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKyuchukov, H., & De Villiers, J. ( 2009 ). Theory of mind and evidentiality in Romani- Bulgarian bilingual children. Psychology of Language and Communication, 13, 21 - 34. https://doi.org/10.2478/v10057- 009- 0007- 4
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLynch, A. ( 2017 ). Bilingualism and second language acquisition. In N. Van Deusen- Scholl & S. May (Eds.), Second and foreign language education (Vol. 4, pp. 43 - 55 ). Springer International. https://doi.org/10.1007/978- 3- 319- 02246- 8_5
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMeir, N., & Novogrodsky, R. ( 2019 ). Prerequisites of third- person pronoun use in monolingual and bilingual children with autism and typical language development. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1 - 14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02289
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNguyen, T.- K., & Astington, J. W. ( 2014 ). Reassessing the bilingual advantage in theory of mind and its cognitive underpinnings. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 396 - 409. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000394
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNichols, E. S., Wild, C. J., Stojanoski, B., Battista, M. E., & Owen, A. M. ( 2020 ). Bilingualism affords no general cognitive advantages: A population study of executive function in 11,000 people. Psychological Science, 31, 548 - 567. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620903113
dc.identifier.citedreferenceOsterhaus, C., Koerber, S., & Sodian, B. ( 2016 ). Scaling of advanced theory- of- mind tasks. Child Development, 87, 1971 - 1991. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12566
dc.identifier.citedreferencePearson, D. K. ( 2013 ). Effect of language background on metalinguistic awareness and theory of mind. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Stirling.
dc.identifier.citedreferencePeristeri, E., Baldimtsi, E., Durrleman, S., & Tsimpli, I. M. ( 2019 ). Bilingualism effects in children with developmental language disorder: Metalinguistic awareness, executive functions, and false- belief reasoning. In M. M. Brown & B. Dailey (Eds.), Proceedings of the 43rd Boston University conference on language development (pp. 523 - 536 ). Somerville: Cascadilla Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferencePetitto, L.- A., & Kovelman, I. ( 2003 ). The bilingual paradox: How signing- speaking bilingual children help us to resolve it and teach us about the brain- s mechanisms underlying all language acquisition. Learning Languages, 8, 5 - 18.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRaisa, Z. H., Karuppali, S., Bhat, J. S., & Bajaj, G. ( 2019 ). Thinking about what he thinks of what I think: Assessing higher theory of mind abilities in Indian bilingual children between 3.0 and 8.11 years of age. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 61, 167 - 176. https://doi.org/10.4103/psychiatry.IndianJPsychiatry_115_18
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTare, M., & Gelman, S. A. ( 2010 ). Can you say it another way? Cognitive factors in bilingual children- s pragmatic language skills. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11, 137 - 158. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248371003699951
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTran, C. D., Arredondo, M. M., & Yoshida, H. ( 2019 ). Early executive function: The influence of culture and bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22, 714 - 732. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000160
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. ( 2001 ). Meta- analysis of theory- of- mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655 - 684. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467- 8624.00304
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWellman, H. M., & Liu, D. ( 2004 ). Scaling of theory- of- mind tasks. Child Development, 75, 523 - 541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- 8624.2004.00691.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBanasik, N., & PodsiadÅ o, K. ( 2016 ). Comprehension of ironic utterances by bilingual children. Psychology of Language and Communication, 20, 316 - 335. https://doi.org/10.1515/plc- 2016- 0019
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBen- Zeev, S. ( 1977 ). The influence of bilingualism on cognitive strategy and cognitive development. Child Development, 48, 1009 - 1018. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128353
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBerguno, G., & Bowler, D. M. ( 2004 ). Communicative interactions, knowledge of a second language, and theory of mind in young children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 165, 293 - 309. https://doi.org/10.3200/GNTP.165.3.293- 309
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. ( 2012 ). Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 240 - 250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBialystok, E., & Senman, L. ( 2004 ). Executive processes in appearance- reality tasks: The role of inhibition of attention and symbolic representation. Child Development, 75, 562 - 579. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- 8624.2004.00693.x
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBuac, M., & Kaushanskaya, M. ( 2020 ). Predictors of theory of mind performance in bilingual and monolingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 24, 339 - 359. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006919826866
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChan, K. T. ( 2004 ). Chinese- English bilinguals- theory- of- mind development. University of Toronto.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCheung, H., Mak, W. Y., Luo, X., & Xiao, W. ( 2010 ). Sociolinguistic awareness and false belief in young Cantonese learners of English. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 188 - 194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.05.001
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChoi, J. Y., Jeon, S., & Lippard, C. ( 2018 ). Dual language learning, inhibitory control, and math achievement in Head Start and kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 42, 66 - 78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.09.001
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDahlgren, S., Almén, H., & Dahlgren Sandberg, A. ( 2017 ). Theory of mind and executive functions in young bilingual children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 178, 303 - 307. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2017.1361376
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDevine, R. T., & Hughes, C. ( 2014 ). Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta- analysis. Child Development, 85, 1777 - 1794. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12237
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDiaz, V., & Farrar, M. J. ( 2018a ). The missing explanation of the false- belief advantage in bilingual children: A longitudinal study. Developmental Science, 21, e12594. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12594
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDiaz, V., & Farrar, M. J. ( 2018b ). Do bilingual and monolingual preschoolers acquire false belief understanding similarly? The role of executive functioning and language. First Language, 38, 382 - 398. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723717752741
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchroeder, S. R. ( 2018 ). Do bilinguals have an advantage in theory of mind? A meta- analysis. Frontiers in Communication, 3, 1 - 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2018.00036
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDick, A. S., Garcia, N. L., Pruden, S. M., Thompson, W. K., Hawes, S. W., Sutherland, M. T., Riedel, M. C., Laird, A. R., & Gonzalez, R. ( 2019 ). No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the ABCD study. Nature Human Behaviour, 3, 692 - 701. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562- 019- 0609- 3
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDoherty, M., & Perner, J. ( 1998 ). Metalinguistic awareness and theory of mind: Just two words for the same thing? Cognitive Development, 13, 279 - 305. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885- 2014(98)90012- 0
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFan, S. P., Liberman, Z., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D. ( 2015 ). The exposure advantage. Psychological Science, 26, 1090 - 1097. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615574699
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFarhadian, M., Abdullah, R., Mansor, M., Redzuan, M., Gazanizadand, N., & Kumar, V. ( 2010 ). Theory of mind in bilingual and monolingual preschool children. Journal of Psychology, 1, 39 - 46. https://doi.org/10.1080/09764224.2010.11885444
dc.working.doiNOen
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.