Show simple item record

Brain bases of English morphological processing: A comparison between Chinese-English, Spanish-English bilingual, and English monolingual children

dc.contributor.authorSun, Xin
dc.contributor.authorMarks, Rebecca A.
dc.contributor.authorZhang, Kehui
dc.contributor.authorYu, Chi-Lin
dc.contributor.authorEggleston, Rachel L.
dc.contributor.authorNickerson, Nia
dc.contributor.authorChou, Tai-Li
dc.contributor.authorHu, Xiao-Su
dc.contributor.authorTardif, Twila
dc.contributor.authorSatterfield, Teresa
dc.contributor.authorKovelman, Ioulia
dc.date.accessioned2023-01-11T16:22:10Z
dc.date.available2024-02-11 11:22:09en
dc.date.available2023-01-11T16:22:10Z
dc.date.issued2023-01
dc.identifier.citationSun, Xin; Marks, Rebecca A.; Zhang, Kehui; Yu, Chi-Lin ; Eggleston, Rachel L.; Nickerson, Nia; Chou, Tai-Li ; Hu, Xiao-Su ; Tardif, Twila; Satterfield, Teresa; Kovelman, Ioulia (2023). "Brain bases of English morphological processing: A comparison between Chinese- English, Spanish- English bilingual, and English monolingual children." Developmental Science 26(1): n/a-n/a.
dc.identifier.issn1363-755X
dc.identifier.issn1467-7687
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/175407
dc.description.abstractHow do early bilingual experiences influence children’s neural architecture for word processing? Dual language acquisition can yield common influences that may be shared across different bilingual groups, as well as language-specific influences stemming from a given language pairing. To investigate these effects, we examined bilingual English speakers of Chinese or Spanish, and English monolinguals, all raised in the US (N = 152, ages 5–10). Children completed an English morphological word processing task during fNIRS neuroimaging. The findings revealed both language-specific and shared bilingual effects. The language-specific effects were that Chinese and Spanish bilinguals showed principled differences in their neural organization for English lexical morphology. The common bilingual effects shared by the two groups were that in both bilingual groups, increased home language proficiency was associated with stronger left superior temporal gyrus (STG) activation when processing the English word structures that are most dissimilar from the home language. The findings inform theories of language and brain development during the key periods of neural reorganization for learning to read by illuminating experience-based plasticity in linguistically diverse learners.
dc.publisherWiley Periodicals, Inc.
dc.publisherErlbaum
dc.subject.otherSpanish-English bilingual
dc.subject.otherfNIRS
dc.subject.otherChinese-English bilingual
dc.subject.otherbrain development
dc.subject.otherbilingualism
dc.subject.othermorphological processing
dc.titleBrain bases of English morphological processing: A comparison between Chinese-English, Spanish-English bilingual, and English monolingual children
dc.typeArticle
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollow
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPediatrics
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelHealth Sciences
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Reviewed
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/175407/1/desc13251_am.pdf
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/175407/2/desc13251.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/desc.13251
dc.identifier.sourceDevelopmental Science
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMarks, R. A., Eggleston, R. L., Sun, X., Yu, C.-L., Zhang, K., Nickerson, N., Hu, X.-S. H., & Kovelman, I. ( 2021 ). The neurocognitive basis of morphological processing in typical and impaired readers. Annals of Dyslexia, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-021-00239-9
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLeonard, M. K., & Chang, E. F. ( 2014 ). Dynamic speech representations in the human temporal lobe. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18 ( 9 ), 472 – 479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.05.001
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLiebenthal, E., Binder, J. R., Spitzer, S. M., Possing, E. T., & Medler, D. A. ( 2005 ). Neural substrates of phonemic perception. Cerebral Cortex, 15 ( 10 ), 1621 – 1631. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhi040
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLiu, H., & Cao, F. ( 2016 ). L1 and L2 processing in the bilingual brain: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Brain and Language, 159, 60 – 73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.05.013
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLiu, P. D., & McBride-Chang, C. ( 2010 ). What is morphological awareness? Tapping lexical compounding awareness in Chinese third graders. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102 ( 1 ), 62 – 73. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016933
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLouleli, N., Hämäläinen, J. A., Nieminen, L., Parviainen, T., & Leppänen, P. H. T. ( 2020 ). Dynamics of morphological processing in pre-school children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56, 100931. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100931
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLlorente, M. A. ( 2013 ). A comparative Aanalysis between English and Spanish native speakers’ production and comprehension of N-N compounds. ES Review. Spanish Journal of English Studies, 34, 7 – 40. https://revistas.uva.es/index.php/esreview/article/view/2172
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLu, L., & Liu, H. S. ( 1998 ). The peabody picture vocabulary test-revised in Chinese. Psychological Publishing.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLuo, Y. C., Chen, X., & Geva, E. ( 2014 ). Concurrent and longitudinal cross-linguistic transfer of phonological awareness and morphological awareness in Chinese-English bilingual children. Written Language and Literacy, 17 ( 1 ), 89 – 115. https://doi.org/10.1075/wll.17.1.05luo
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMarks, R. A., Labotka, D., Sun, X., Nickerson, N., Zhang, K., Eggleston, R. L., Yu, C., Hoeft, F. U. Y., & Kovelman, I. ( 2021 ). Morphological awareness contributes to early literacy in linguistically diverse readers. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/xpycj
dc.identifier.citedreferencePasquarella, A., Chen, X., Lam, K., Luo, Y. C., & Ramírez, G. ( 2011 ). Cross-language transfer of morphological awareness in Chinese–English bilinguals. Journal of Research in Reading, 34 ( 1 ), 23 – 42. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01484.x
dc.identifier.citedreferencePetitto, L. A., & Kovelman, I. ( 2003 ). The bilingual paradox: How signing-speaking bilingual children help us to resolve it and teach us about the brain’s mechanisms underlying all language acquisition. Learning Languages, 8 ( 3 ), 5 – 18.
dc.identifier.citedreferencePoulin-Dubois, D., Bialystok, E., Blaye, A., Polonia, A., & Yott, J. ( 2013 ). Lexical access and vocabulary development in very young bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17 ( 1 ), 57 – 70. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006911431198
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRamírez, G., Chen, X., Geva, E., & Kiefer, H. ( 2010 ). Morphological awareness in Spanish-speaking English language learners: Within and cross-language effects on word reading. Reading and Writing, 23 ( 3 ), 337 – 358. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9203-9
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRamírez, G., Chen, X., Geva, E., & Luo, Y. ( 2011 ). Morphological awareness and word reading in English language learners: Evidence from Spanish-and Chinese-speaking children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32 ( 3 ), 601 – 618. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716411000233
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRupawala, M., Dehghani, H., Lucas, S. J., Tino, P., & Cruse, D. ( 2018 ). Shining a light on awareness: A review of functional near-infrared spectroscopy for prolonged disorders of consciousness. Frontiers in Neurology, 9 ( 350 ),. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2018.00350
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSantosa, H., Zhai, X., Fishburn, F., & Huppert, T. ( 2018 ). The NIRS brain AnalyzIR toolbox. Algorithms, 11 ( 5 ), 73. https://doi.org/10.3390/a11050073
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSatterfield, T., & Rusty, B. ( 2004 ). Generation Gap: Explaining new and emerging word-order phenomena in Mayan-Spanish bilinguals. Proceedings of the First International Symposium on Bilingualism in Latin America. 285 – 300. http://www-personal.umich.edu/~tsatter/ESSARP04.pdf
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchrank, F. A., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. ( 2014 ). Woodcock-Johnson IV. Riverside.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceShook, A., & Marian, V. ( 2019 ). Covert co-activation of bilinguals’ non-target language: Phonological competition from translations. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 9 ( 2 ), 228 – 252. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.17022.sho
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStrangman, G., Culver, J. P., Thompson, J. H., & Boas, D. A. ( 2002 ). A quantitative comparison of simultaneous BOLD fMRI and NIRS recordings during functional brain activation. Neuroimage, 17 ( 2 ), 719 – 731. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1227
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSun, X., Zhang, K., Marks, R., Nickerson, N., Eggleston, R., Yu, C. L., Chou, T., Tardif, T., & Kovelman, I. ( 2021 ). What’s in a word? Cross-linguistic influences on Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilingual children’s word reading development. Child Development, https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13666
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTong, X., Chung, K. K. H., & McBride, C. ( 2014 ). Two-character Chinese compound word processing in Chinese children with and without dyslexia: ERP evidence. Developmental Neuropsychology, 39 ( 4 ), 285 – 301. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2014.907720
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTong, X., Deacon, S. H., & Cain, K. ( 2014 ). Morphological and syntactic awareness in poor comprehenders: Another piece of the puzzle. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47 ( 1 ), 22 – 33. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509971
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTong, X., Deacon, S. H., Kirby, J. R., Cain, K., & Parilla, R. ( 2011 ). Morphological awareness: A key to understanding poor reading comprehension in English. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103 ( 3 ), 523 – 534. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023495
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTong, X., Tong, X., & McBride, C. ( 2017 ). Unpacking the relation between morphological awareness and Chinese word reading: Levels of morphological awareness and vocabulary. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 48, 167 – 178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.07.003
dc.identifier.citedreferenceVannest, J., Newport, E. L., Newman, A. J., & Bavelier, D. ( 2011 ). Interplay between morphology and frequency in lexical access: The case of the base frequency effect. Brain Research, 1373, 144 – 159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.12.022
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., & Pearson, N. A. ( 1999 ). Comprehensive test of phonological processing: CTOPP. Pro-ed.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWang, M., Cheng, C. X., & Chen, S. W. ( 2006 ). Contribution of morphological awareness to Chinese-English biliteracy acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98 ( 3 ), 542 – 553. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.542
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWang, J., Yamasaki, B. L., Weiss, Y., & Booth, J. R. ( 2021 ). Both frontal and temporal cortex exhibit phonological and semantic specialization during spoken language processing in 7-to 8-year-old children. Human Brain Mapping, 42 ( 11 ): 3534 – 3546. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25450
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWerker, J. F., & Hensch, T. K. ( 2015 ). Critical periods in speech perception: New directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 66 ( 1 ), 173 – 196. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015104
dc.identifier.citedreferenceZwanziger, E. E., Allen, S. E., & Genesee, F. ( 2005 ). Crosslinguistic influence in bilingual acquisition: Subject omission in learners of Inuktitut and English. Journal of Child Language, 32 ( 4 ), 893 – 909. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000905007129
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAbutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. ( 2016 ). Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: Neural adaptation and reserve. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 19 ( 4 ), 689 – 698. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000225
dc.identifier.citedreferenceArredondo, M. M., Hu, X. S., Satterfield, T., & Kovelman, I. ( 2017 ). Bilingualism alters children’s frontal lobe functioning for attentional control. Developmental Science, 20 ( 3 ), e12377. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12377
dc.identifier.citedreferenceArredondo, M. M., Hu, X. S., Satterfield, T., Riobóo, A. T., Gelman, S. A., & Kovelman, I. ( 2019 ). Bilingual effects on lexical selection: A neurodevelopmental perspective. Brain and Language, 195, 104640. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2019.104640
dc.identifier.citedreferenceArredondo, M. M., Ip, K. I., Shih Ju Hsu, L., Tardif, T., & Kovelman, I. ( 2015 ). Brain bases of morphological processing in young children. Human Brain Mapping, 36 ( 8 ), 2890 – 2900. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22815
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAylward, E. H., Richards, T. L., Berninger, V. W., Nagy, W. E., Field, K. M., Grimme, A. C., Richards, A. L., Thomson, J. B., & Cramer, S. C. ( 2003 ). Instructional treatment associated with changes in brain activation in children with dyslexia. Neurology, 61 ( 2 ), 212 – 219. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.WNL.0000068363.05974.64
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBarker, J. W., Aarabi, A., & Huppert, T. J. ( 2013 ). Autoregressive model based algorithm for correcting motion and serially correlated errors in fNIRS. Biomedical Optics Express, 4 ( 8 ), 1366 – 1379. https://doi.org/10.1364/BOE.4.001366
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBinder, J. R. ( 2017 ). Current controversies on Wernicke’s area and its role in language. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 17, 58. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-017-0764-8
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCargnelutti, E., Tomasino, B., & Fabbro, F. ( 2019 ). Language brain representation in bilinguals with different age of appropriation and proficiency of the second language: A meta-analysis of functional imaging studies. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, 154. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00154
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarlisle, J. F. ( 1995 ). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In ( L. Feldman Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189 – 209 ). Erlbaum.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChee, M. W., Soon, C. S., & Lee, H. L. ( 2003 ). Common and segregated neuronal networks for different languages revealed using functional magnetic resonance adaptation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15 ( 1 ), 85 – 97. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903321107846
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChung, S. C., Chen, X., & Geva, E. ( 2019 ). Deconstructing and reconstructing cross-language transfer in bilingual reading development: An interactive framework. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 50, 149 – 161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2018.01.003
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCosta, A., & Caramazza, A. ( 1999 ). Is lexical selection in bilingual speech production language-specific? Further evidence from Spanish–English and English–Spanish bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 2 ( 3 ), 231 – 244. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728999000334
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCosta, A., & Caramazza, A. ( 2000 ). Lexical access in speech production: The bilingual case. Psicológica, 21 ( 2 ), 403 – 437. https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=16921211
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCosta, A., Santesteban, M., & Caño, A. ( 2005 ). On the facilitatory effects of cognate words in bilingual speech production. Brain and Language, 94 ( 1 ), 94 – 103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2004.12.002
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDas, T., Padakannaya, P., Pugh, K. R., & Singh, N. C. ( 2011 ). Neuroimaging reveals dual routes to reading in simultaneous proficient readers of two orthographies. Neuroimage, 54 ( 2 ), 1476 – 1487. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.022
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDavies, M. ( 2020 ). The Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA). Available online at https://www.english-corpora.org/coca/
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDeLuca, V., Segaert, K., Mazaheri, A., & Krott, A. ( 2020 ). Understanding bilingual brain function and structure changes? U Bet! A Unified Bilingual Experience Trajectory model. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56, 100930. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100930
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDevlin, J. T., Jamison, H. L., Matthews, P. M., & Gonnerman, L. M. ( 2004 ). Morphology and the internal structure of words. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101 ( 41 ), 14984 – 14988. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0403766101
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDijkstra, T., Wahl, A., Buytenhuijs, F., Van Halem, N., Al-Jibouri, Z., De Korte, M., & Rekké, S. ( 2019 ). Multilink: A computational model for bilingual word recognition and word translation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22 ( 4 ), 657 – 679. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000287
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDunn, D. M. ( 2019 ). Peabody picture vocabulary test 5. NCS Pearson.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDunn, L., Padilla, E., Lugo, D., & Dunn, L. ( 1986 ). TVIP: Test vocabolario imágenes peabody. American Guidance Service.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEnge, A., Friederici, A. D., & Skeide, M. A. ( 2020 ). A meta-analysis of fMRI studies of language comprehension in children. Neuroimage, 215, 116858. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116858
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEttinger, A., Linzen, T., & Marantz, A. ( 2014 ). The role of morphology in phoneme prediction: Evidence from MEG. Brain and Language, 129, 14 – 23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.11.004
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFiorentino, R., & Poeppel, D. ( 2007 ). Compound words and structure in the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22 ( 7 ), 953 – 1000. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960701190215
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFriederici, A. D., & Gierhan, S. M. ( 2013 ). The language network. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23 ( 2 ), 250 – 254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.10.002
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFriederici, A. D., Rüschemeyer, S. A., Hahne, A., & Fiebach, C. J. ( 2003 ). The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: Localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cerebral Cortex, 13 ( 2 ), 170 – 177. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/13.2.170
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFriston, K. J., Ashburner, J., Kiebel, S. J., Nichols, T., & Penny, W. ( 2007 ). Statistical parametric mapping: The analysis of functional brain images. Academic Press.
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGagnon, L., Yücel, M. A., Dehaes, M., Cooper, R. J., Perdue, K. L., Selb, J., Huppert, T. J., Hoge, R. D., & Boas, D. A. ( 2012 ). Quantification of the cortical contribution to the NIRS signal over the motor cortex using concurrent NIRS-fMRI measurements. Neuroimage, 59 ( 4 ), 3933 – 3940. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.054
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGoh, W. D., Yap, M. J., & Chee, Q. W. ( 2020 ). The Auditory English Lexicon Project: A multi-talker, multi-region psycholinguistic database of 10,170 spoken words and nonwords. Behavior Research Methods, 52 ( 5 ), 2202 – 2231. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01352-0
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGwilliams, L. ( 2020 ). How the brain composes morphemes into meaning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375 ( 1791 ). Article 20190311. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0311
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHagoort, P. ( 2005 ). On Broca, brain, and binding: A new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9 ( 9 ), 416 – 423. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.004
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHagoort, P. ( 2019 ). The neurobiology of language beyond single-word processing. Science, 366 ( 6461 ), 55 – 58. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aax0289
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHernandez, A. E., Ronderos, J., Bodet, J. P., Claussenius-Kalman, H., Nguyen, M. V., & Bunta, F. ( 2021 ). German in childhood and Latin in adolescence: On the bidialectal nature of lexical access in English. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 8 ( 1 ), 1 – 12. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-021-00836-4
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHickok, G., & Poeppel, D. ( 2007 ). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8 ( 5 ), 393 – 402. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2113
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHoshi, Y. ( 2007 ). Functional near-infrared spectroscopy: Current status and future prospects. Journal of Biomedical Optics, 12 ( 6 ), 062106. https://doi.org/10.1117/1.2804911
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHu, X. S., Wagley, N., Rioboo, A. T., DaSilva, A. F., & Kovelman, I. ( 2020 ). Photogrammetry-based stereoscopic optode registration method for functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Journal of Biomedical Optics, 25 ( 9 ), 095001. https://doi.org/10.1117/1.JBO.25.9.095001
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHuppert, T. J., Karim, H., Lin, C. C., Alqahtani, B. A., Greenspan, S. L., & Sparto, P. J. ( 2017 ). Functional imaging of cognition in an old-old population: A case for portable functional near-infrared spectroscopy. PloS One, 12 ( 10 ), e0184918. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184918
dc.identifier.citedreferenceIp, K. I., Hsu, L. S. J., Arredondo, M. M., Tardif, T., & Kovelman, I. ( 2017 ). Brain bases of morphological processing in Chinese-English bilingual children. Developmental Science, 20 ( 5 ), e12449. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12449
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJasinska, K. K., Berens, M. S., Kovelman, I., & Petitto, L. A. ( 2017 ). Bilingualism yields language-specific plasticity in left hemisphere’s circuitry for learning to read in young children. Neuropsychologia, 98, 34 – 45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.018
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJasinska, K. K., & Petitto, L. A. ( 2013 ). How age of bilingual exposure can change the neural systems for language in the developing brain: A functional near infrared spectroscopy investigation of syntactic processing in monolingual and bilingual children. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 87 – 101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2013.06.005
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJasińska, K. K., & Petitto, L. A. ( 2014 ). Development of neural systems for reading in the monolingual and bilingual brain: New insights from functional near infrared spectroscopy neuroimaging. Developmental Neuropsychology, 39 ( 6 ), 421 – 439. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2014.939180
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKasparian, K., Vespignani, F., & Steinhauer, K. ( 2017 ). First language attrition induces changes in online morphosyntactic processing and re-analysis: An ERP study of number agreement in complex Italian sentences. Cognitive Science, 41 ( 7 ), 1760 – 1803. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12450
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKe, S., Miller, R. T., Zhang, D., & Koda, K. ( 2021 ). Crosslinguistic sharing of morphological awareness in biliteracy development: A systematic review and meta-analysis of correlation coefficients. Language Learning, 71 ( 1 ), 8 – 54. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12429
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKirby, J. R., & Bowers, P. N. ( 2017 ). Morphological instruction and literacy: Binding phonological, orthographic, and semantic features of words. In ( K. Cain, D. L. Compton, & R. K. Parrila Eds.), Studies in written language and literacy (Vol., 15, pp. 437 – 462 ). John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://doi.org/10.1075/swll.15.24kir
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKovelman, I., Baker, S. A., & Petitto, L. A. ( 2008 ). Age of first bilingual language exposure as a new window into bilingual reading development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11 ( 2 ), 203 – 223. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728908003386
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKovelman, I., Shalinsky, M. H., White, K. S., Schmitt, S. N., Berens, M. S., Paymer, N., & Petitto, L. A. ( 2009 ). Dual language use in sign-speech bimodal bilinguals: FNIRS brain-imaging evidence. Brain and Language, 109 ( 2-3 ), 112 – 123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2008.09.008
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKroll, J. F., Van Hell, J. G., Tokowicz, N., & Green, D. W. ( 2010 ). The revised hierarchical model: A critical review and assessment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13 ( 3 ), 373 – 381. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891000009X
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKuo, L. J., Ramírez, G., de Marin, S., Kim, T. J., & Unal-Gezer, M. ( 2017 ). Bilingualism and morphological awareness: A study with children from general education and Spanish-English dual language programs. Educational Psychology, 37 ( 2 ), 94 – 111. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2015.1049586
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLeminen, A., Smolka, E., Dunabeitia, J. A., & Pliatsikas, C. ( 2019 ). Morphological processing in the brain: The good (inflection), the bad (derivation) and the ugly (compounding). Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 116, 4 – 44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.08.016
dc.working.doiNOen
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.