Semiotics in the head: Thinking about and thinking through symbols
dc.contributor.author | Munroe, Wade | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2023-11-06T16:36:00Z | |
dc.date.available | 2024-10-06 11:35:58 | en |
dc.date.available | 2023-11-06T16:36:00Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2023-09 | |
dc.identifier.citation | Munroe, Wade (2023). "Semiotics in the head: Thinking about and thinking through symbols." Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 107(2): 413-438. | |
dc.identifier.issn | 0031-8205 | |
dc.identifier.issn | 1933-1592 | |
dc.identifier.uri | https://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/191391 | |
dc.description.abstract | Our conscious thought, at least at times, seems suffused with language. We may experience thinking as if we were “talking in our head”, thus using inner speech to verbalize, e.g., our premises, lemmas, and conclusions. I take inner speech to be part of a larger phenomenon I call inner semiotics, where inner semiotics involves the subjective experience of expressions in a semiotic (or symbol) system absent the overt articulation of the expressions. In this paper, I argue that inner semiotics allows us to bootstrap our way into entertaining thoughts about exact numbers and quantities that we couldn’t prior to our competence with a numeric code. I establish that our arithmetic thoughts literally occur as (internal ‘articulations’ of) expressions in a numeric code. However, a problem arises for my view: just as we can slip in overt speech, producing an utterance that deviates from what we mean to say, there is very good evidence that we can slip in inner speech as well. If our arithmetic thought occurs in a numeric code, it’s far from clear how we determine when a covert utterance constitutes a slip. In closing, I provide an account of what makes an inner speech utterance a slip. | |
dc.publisher | Wiley Periodicals, Inc. | |
dc.publisher | Oxford University Press | |
dc.title | Semiotics in the head: Thinking about and thinking through symbols | |
dc.type | Article | |
dc.rights.robots | IndexNoFollow | |
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevel | Philosophy | |
dc.subject.hlbtoplevel | Humanities | |
dc.description.peerreviewed | Peer Reviewed | |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/191391/1/phpr12923_am.pdf | |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/191391/2/phpr12923.pdf | |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1111/phpr.12923 | |
dc.identifier.source | Philosophy and Phenomenological Research | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Noel, M.-P., & Seron, X. ( 1993 ). Arabic number reading deficit: A single case study or when 236 is read (2306) and judged superior to 1258. Cognitive neuropsychology, 10 ( 4 ), 317 – 339. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Spaepen, E., Coppola, M., Spelke, E. S., Carey, S. E., & Goldin-Meadow, S. ( 2011 ). Number without a language model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108 ( 8 ), 3163 – 3168. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Tanaka, S., Seki, K., Hanakawa, T., Harada, M., Sugawara, S. K., Sadato, N., … Honda, M. ( 2012 ). Abacus in the Brain: A Longitudinal Functional MRI Study of a Skilled Abacus User with a Right Hemispheric Lesion. Frontiers in psychology, 3, 1 – 13. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Tian, X., & Poeppel, D. ( 2012 ). Mental imagery of speech: linking motor and perceptual systems through internal simulation and estimation. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 6, 314. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. ( 1980 ). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive psychology, 12 ( 1 ), 97 – 136. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Van Rinsveld, A., Dricot, L., Guillaume, M., Rossion, B., & Schiltz, C. ( 2017 ). Mental arithmetic in the bilingual brain: Language matters. Neuropsychologia, 101, 17 – 29. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Vicente, A., & Jorba, M. ( 2019 ). The linguistic determination of conscious thought contents. Noûs, 53 ( 3 ), 737 – 759. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Vicente, A., & Martínez-Manrique, F. ( 2016 ). The nature of unsymbolized thinking. Philosophical Explorations, 19 ( 2 ), 173 – 187. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Washington, C. ( 1992 ). The identity theory of quotation. Journal of Philosophy, 89 ( 11 ), 582 – 605. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Whitford, T. J., Jack, B. N., Pearson, D., Griffiths, O., Luque, D., Harris, A. W., … Le Pelley, M. E. ( 2017 ). Neurophysiological evidence of efference copies to inner speech. Elife, 6, 1 – 26. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wilkinson, S., & Fernyhough, C. ( 2017 ). Auditory Verbal Hallucinations and Inner Speech: A Predictive Processing Perspective. In Z. Radman (Ed.), Before Consciousness: In Search of the Fundamentals of Mind. Exeter (UK): Imprint Academic. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Williams, J. R. G. ( 2020 ). The metaphysics of representation: Oxford University Press, USA. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wilson, M. ( 2001 ). The case for sensorimotor coding in working memory. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 8 ( 1 ), 44 – 57. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wilson, M. L., Hauser, M. D., & Wrangham, R. W. ( 2001 ). Does participation in intergroup conflict depend on numerical assessment, range location, or rank for wild chimpanzees? Animal Behaviour, 61 ( 6 ), 1203 – 1216. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wynn, K. ( 1992 ). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358 ( 6389 ), 749 – 750. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Yang, C. ( 2016 ). The linguistic origin of the next number. LingBuzz preprint lingbuzz/003824. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Zhang, X., Koponen, T., Räsänen, P., Aunola, K., Lerkkanen, M. K., & Nurmi, J. E. ( 2014 ). Linguistic and spatial skills predict early arithmetic development via counting sequence knowledge. Child development, 85 ( 3 ), 1091 – 1107. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Alderson-Day, B., & Fernyhough, C. ( 2015 ). Inner Speech: Development, Cognitive Functions, Phenomenology, and Neurobiology. Psychological bulletin, 141 ( 5 ), 931 – 965. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ardila, A., & Rosselli, M. ( 1990 ). Acalculias. Behavioural Neurology, 3 ( 1 ), 39 – 48. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ardila, A., & Rosselli, M. ( 2002 ). Acalculia and dyscalculia. Neuropsychology review, 12 ( 4 ), 179 – 231. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ball, B. ( 2017 ). On representational content and format in core numerical cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 30 ( 1-2 ), 119 – 139. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Beran, M. J. ( 2001 ). Summation and numerousness judgments of sequentially presented sets of items by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115 ( 2 ), 181 – 191. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Beran, M. J. ( 2004 ). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) respond to nonvisible sets after one-by-one addition and removal of items. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118 ( 1 ), 25 – 36. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Beran, M. J., & Beran, M. M. ( 2004 ). Chimpanzees remember the results of one-by-one addition of food items to sets over extended time periods. Psychological science, 15 ( 2 ), 94 – 99. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bermúdez, J. L. ( 2003 ). Thinking Without Words. New York: Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Berteletti, I., & Booth, J. R. ( 2016 ). Finger Representation and Finger-Based Strategies in the Acquisition of Number Meaning and Arithmetic. In D. B. Berch, D. C. Geary, & K. M. Koepke (Eds.), Development of Mathematical Cognition (pp. 109 – 139 ). San Diego: Academic Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Borghi, A. M., & Binkofski, F. ( 2014 ). Words as social tools: An embodied view on abstract concepts. New York: Springer. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Brandom, R. ( 1994 ). Making it Explicit: Reasoning, Representing, and Discursive Commitment. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard university press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Brannon, E. M., Jordan, K. E., & Jones, S. M. ( 2010 ). Behavioral Signatures of Numerical Cognition. In M. Platt & A. Ghazanfar (Eds.), Primate neuroethology (pp. 144 – 159 ): Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Butterworth, B., Reeve, R., Reynolds, F., & Lloyd, D. ( 2008 ). Numerical thought with and without words: Evidence from indigenous Australian children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105 ( 35 ), 13179 – 13184. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cajori, F. ( 1993 ). A history of mathematical notations. Mineola, New York: Dover Publications. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cappelen, H., & Lepore, E. ( 2007 ). Language turned on itself: The semantics and pragmatics of metalinguistic discourse: Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Caramazza, A., & McCloskey, M. ( 1987 ). Dissociations of Calculation Processes. In G. Deloche & X. Seron (Eds.), Mathematical Disabilities: A Cognitive Neuropsychological Perspective (pp. 221 – 234 ). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Carruthers, P. ( 1998 ). Conscious thinking: Language or elimination? Mind and language, 13 ( 4 ), 457 – 476. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Carruthers, P. ( 2002 ). The cognitive functions of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25 ( 6 ), 657 – 674. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Carruthers, P. ( 2006 ). The architecture of the mind: Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Carruthers, P. ( 2011 ). The Opacity of Mind: An Integrative Theory of Self-Knowledge: Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Carruthers, P. ( 2015 ). The centered mind: What the science of working memory shows us about the nature of human thought: Oxfrod University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Carruthers, P. ( 2018 ). The causes and contents of inner speech. In P. Langland-Hassan & A. Vicente (Eds.), Inner Speech: New Voices (pp. 31 – 52 ): Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cipolotti, L., & Butterworth, B. ( 1995 ). Toward a multiroute model of number processing: Impaired number transcoding with preserved calculation skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124 ( 4 ), 375 – 390. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cipolotti, L., Warrington, E. K., & Butterworth, B. ( 1995 ). Selective impairment in manipulating Arabic numerals. Cortex, 31 ( 1 ), 73 – 86. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Clark, A. ( 2006 ). Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10 ( 8 ), 370 – 374. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Clarke, S., & Beck, J. ( 2021 ). The number sense represents (rational) numbers. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 44, e178. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525%D7;21000571 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cohen, L., & Dahaene, S. ( 1995 ). Towards an anatomical and functional model of number processing. Mathematical cognition, 7 ( 1 ), 83 – 120. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Cowey, A. ( 1985 ). Disturbances of Stereopsis by Brain Damage. In D. J. Ingle, M. Jeannerod, & D. N. Lee (Eds.), Brain Mechanisms and Spatial Vision (pp. 259 – 278 ). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Davidson, D. ( 1975 ). Thought and talk. Mind and language, 1975, 7 – 23. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | De Guerrero, M. C. ( 2005 ). Inner speech—L2: Thinking Words in a Second Language. New York: Springer. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Delazer, M., & Bartha, L. ( 2001 ). Transcoding and calculation in aphasia. Aphasiology, 15 ( 7 ), 649 – 679. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Dell, G. S. ( 1986 ). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological review, 93 ( 3 ), 283 – 321. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Dell, G. S., & Repka, R. J. ( 1992 ). Errors in Inner Speech. In B. J. Baars (Ed.), Experimental Slips and Human Error: Exploring the Architecture of Volition (pp. 237 – 262 ). Boston, MA: Springer US. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Deloche, G., & Seron, X. ( 1982 ). From three to 3: A differential analysis of skills in transcoding quantities between patients with Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia. Brain, 105 ( 4 ), 719 – 733. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Dove, G. ( 2020 ). More than a scaffold: Language is a neuroenhancement. Cognitive neuropsychology, 37 ( 5-6 ), 288 – 311. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Everett, D., Berlin, B., Gonalves, M., Kay, P., Levinson, S., Pawley, A., … Everett, D. ( 2005 ). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: Another look at the design features of human language. Current anthropology, 46 ( 4 ), 621 – 646. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fama, M. E., & Turkeltaub, P. E. ( 2020 ). Inner Speech in Aphasia: Current Evidence, Clinical Implications, and Future Directions. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29 ( 1S ), 560 – 573. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fayol, M., Barrouillet, P., & Marinthe, C. ( 1998 ). Predicting Arithmetical Achievement from Neuro-Psychological Performance: A Longitudinal Study. Cognition, 68 ( 2 ), B63 - B70. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. ( 2004 ). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8 ( 7 ), 307 – 314. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fernyhough, C. ( 2004 ). Alien voices and inner dialogue: towards a developmental account of auditory verbal hallucinations. New ideas in Psychology, 22 ( 1 ), 49 – 68. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Frank, M. C., & Barner, D. ( 2012 ). Representing Exact Number Visually Using Mental Abacus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141 ( 1 ), 134 – 139. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Frank, M. C., Everett, D. L., Fedorenko, E., & Gibson, E. ( 2008 ). Number as a cognitive technology: Evidence from Pirahã language and cognition. Cognition, 108 ( 3 ), 819 – 824. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Frank, M. C., Fedorenko, E., & Gibson, E. ( 2008 ). Language as a Cognitive Technology: English-Speakers Match Like Pirahã When You Don’t Let Them Count. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 30, 439 – 444. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Frankish, K. ( 2004 ). Mind and supermind: Cambridge University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Frankish, K. ( 2009 ). Systems and levels: Dual-system theories and the personalsubpersonal distinction. In J. S. B. T. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 89 – 107 ). New York: Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Frankish, K. ( 2012 ). Dual systems and dual attitudes. Mind & Society, 11 ( 1 ), 41 – 51. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Frankish, K. ( 2018 ). Inner Speech and Outer Thought. In P. Langland-Hassan & A. Vicente (Eds.), Inner Speech: New Voices (pp. 221 – 243 ): Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fuson, K. C. ( 2020 ). An analysis of the counting-on solution procedure in addition. In T. P. Carpenter, J. M. Moser, & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Addition and subtraction (pp. 67 – 81 ): Routledge. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gauker, C. ( 2011 ). Words and Images: An Essay on the Origin of Ideas. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gauker, C. ( 2018 ). Inner Speech as the Internalization of Outer Speech. In P. Langland-Hassan & A. Vicente (Eds.), Inner Speech: New Voices (pp. 53 – 77 ): Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Geva, S., Bennett, S., Warburton, E. A., & Patterson, K. ( 2011 ). Discrepancy between inner and overt speech: Implications for post-stroke aphasia and normal language processing. Aphasiology, 25 ( 3 ), 323 – 343. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gordon, P. ( 2004 ). Numerical cognition without words: Evidence from Amazonia. science, 306 ( 5695 ), 496 – 499. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Grandchamp, R., Rapin, L., Perrone-Bertolotti, M., Pichat, C., Haldin, C., Cousin, E., … Lœvenbruck, H. ( 2019 ). The ConDialInt Model: Condensation, Dialogality, and Intentionality Dimensions of Inner Speech Within a Hierarchical Predictive Control Framework. Frontiers in psychology, 10, 1 – 30. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02019 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gregory, D. ( 2016 ). Inner speech, imagined speech, and auditory verbal hallucinations. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 7 ( 3 ), 653 – 673. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hanakawa, T., Honda, M., Okada, T., Fukuyama, H., & Shibasaki, H. ( 2003 ). Neural Correlates Underlying Mental Calculation in Abacus Experts: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. Neuroimage, 19 ( 2 ), 296 – 307. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hatano, G., Miyake, Y., & Binks, M. G. ( 1977 ). Performance of expert abacus operators. Cognition, 5 ( 1 ), 47 – 55. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Heavey, C. L., & Hurlburt, R. T. ( 2008 ). The Phenomena of Inner Experience. Consciousness and Cognition, 17 ( 3 ), 798 – 810. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hécean, H. ( 1961 ). Les variétés cliniques des acalculies au cours des lésions rétrorolandiques: Approche statistique du probléme. Revue neurologique, 105, 85 – 103. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hittmair-Delazer, M., Sailer, U., & Benke, T. ( 1995 ). Impaired arithmetic facts but intact conceptual knowledge a single—case study of dyscalculia. Cortex, 31 ( 1 ), 139 – 147. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Izard, V., Pica, P., Spelke, E. S., & Dehaene, S. ( 2008 ). Exact equality and successor function: Two key concepts on the path towards understanding exact numbers. Philosophical Psychology, 21 ( 4 ), 491 – 505. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Izard, V., Streri, A., & Spelke, E. S. ( 2014 ). Toward exact number: Young children use one-to-one correspondence to measure set identity but not numerical equality. Cognitive psychology, 72, 27 – 53. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Jackendoff, R. ( 1996 ). How language helps us think. Pragmatics & Cognition, 4 ( 1 ), 1 – 34. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kashiwagi, A., Kashiwagi, T., & Hasegawa, T. ( 1987 ). Improvement of deficits in mnemonic rhyme for multiplication in Japanese aphasics. Neuropsychologia, 25 ( 2 ), 443 – 447. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Koopman, S. E., Arre, A. M., Piantadosi, S. T., & Cantlon, J. F. ( 2019 ). One-to-one correspondence without language. Royal Society Open Science, 6 ( 10 ), 190495. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Krajewski, K., & Schneider, W. ( 2009 ). Early development of quantity to number-word linkage as a precursor of mathematical school achievement and mathematical difficulties: Findings from a four-year longitudinal study. Learning and instruction, 19 (6), 513 – 526. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Langland-Hassan, P. ( 2018 ). From Introspection to Essence: The Auditory Nature of Inner Speech. In P. Langland-Hassan & A. Vicente (Eds.), Inner Speech: New Voices (pp. 78 – 105 ). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Langland-Hassan, P. ( 2021 ). Inner speech. WIREs Cognitive Science, 12 ( 2 ), 1 – 18. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1544 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Langland-Hassan, P., Faries, F. R., Richardson, M. J., & Dietz, A. ( 2015 ). Inner speech deficits in people with aphasia. Frontiers in psychology, 6, 528. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Langland-Hassan, P. ( 2014 ). Inner speech and metacognition: in search of a connection. Mind & Language, 29 ( 5 ), 511 – 533. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lee, K.-M., & Kang, S.-Y. ( 2002 ). Arithmetic operation and working memory: Differential suppression in dual tasks. Cognition, 83 ( 3 ), B63 - B68. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | LeFevre, J.-A., DeStefano, D., Coleman, B., & Shanahan, T. ( 2005 ). Mathematical cognition and working memory. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition. (pp. 361 – 377 ). New York, NY, US: Psychology Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lemaire, P., Barrett, S. E., Fayol, M., & Abdi, H. ( 1994 ). Automatic activation of addition and multiplication facts in elementary school children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57 ( 2 ), 224 – 258. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Levelt, W. J. ( 1989 ). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lewis, D. ( 1970 ). General Semantics. Synthese, 22 ( 1/2 ), 18 – 67. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/20114749 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lœvenbruck, H., Grandchamp, R., Rapin, L., Nalborczyk, L., & Dohen, M. ( 2018 ). A Cognitive Neuroscience View of Inner Language. In P. Langland-Hassan & A. n. Vicente (Eds.), Inner Speech: New Voices (pp. 131 – 167 ). | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ludwig, K. ( 2004 ). Rationality, Language, and the Principle of Charity. In A. R. Mele & P. Rawling (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Rationality: Oup Usa. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lyons, J. ( 2009 ). Perception and Basic Beliefs: Zombies, Modules, and the Problem of the External World: Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Macoir, J., Audet, T., & Breton, M.-F. ( 1999 ). Code-dependent pathways for number transcoding: evidence from a case of selective impairment in written verbal numeral to Arabic transcoding. Cortex, 35 ( 5 ), 629 – 645. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Martínez-Manrique, F., & Vicente, A. ( 2015 ). The activity view of inner speech. Frontiers in psychology, 6, 232. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Miller, J. T. M. ( 2020 ). The ontology of words: Realism, nominalism, and eliminativism. Philosophy Compass, 15 ( 7 ), 1 – 13. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/phc3.12691 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mölder, B. ( 2010 ). Mind Ascribed: An Elaboration and Defence of Interpretivism: John Benjamins Publishing. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Munroe, W. ( 2021 ). Why are you talking to yourself? The epistemic role of inner speech in reasoning. Noûs, 1 – 26. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1111/nous.12385 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Munroe, W. ( 2022 ). Thinking through talking to yourself: Inner speech as a vehicle of conscious reasoning. Philosophical Psychology, 1 – 27. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2022.2042505 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Oppenheim, G. M., & Dell, G. ( 2008 ). Inner speech slips exhibit lexical bias, but not the phonemic similarity effect. Cognition, 106 ( 1 ), 528 – 537. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Patel, S. ( 2021 ). From speech to voice: on the content of inner speech. Synthese, 199 ( 3 ), 10929 – 10952. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-021-03274-6 | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Pautz, A. (forthcoming). The Metaphysics of Representation, by J. Robert, G. Williams. Mind. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Perrone-Bertolotti, M., Rapin, L., Lachaux, J.-P., Baciu, M., & Lœvenbruck, H. ( 2014 ). What is that little voice inside my head? Inner speech phenomenology, its role in cognitive performance, and its relation to self-monitoring. Behavioural brain research, 261, 220 – 239. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V., & Dehaene, S. ( 2004 ). Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. science, 306 ( 5695 ), 499 – 503. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. ( 2013 ). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36 ( 4 ), 329 – 347. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rossor, M. N., Warrington, E. K., & Cipolotti, L. ( 1995 ). The isolation of calculation skills. Journal of neurology, 242 ( 2 ), 78 – 81. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. ( 2007 ). Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nature reviews neuroscience, 8 ( 9 ), 657 – 661. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Seron, X., & Deloche, G. ( 1987 ). The production of counting sequences by aphasics and children: A matter of lexical processing? In X. Seron & G. Deloche (Eds.), Mathematical disabilities: A cognitive neuropsychological perspective. (pp. 171 – 200 ). Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Siegler, R. S. ( 1984 ). Strategy Choices in Addition and Subtraction: How do Children Know What to do? In C. Sophian (Ed.), Origins of Cognitive Skills: The Eighteenth Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition (pp. 229 – 292 ): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. | |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Simmons, F. R., & Singleton, C. ( 2008 ). Do weak phonological representations impact on arithmetic development? A review of research into arithmetic and dyslexia. Dyslexia, 14 ( 2 ), 77 – 94. | |
dc.working.doi | NO | en |
dc.owningcollname | Interdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed |
Files in this item
Remediation of Harmful Language
The University of Michigan Library aims to describe its collections in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in them. We encourage you to Contact Us anonymously if you encounter harmful or problematic language in catalog records or finding aids. More information about our policies and practices is available at Remediation of Harmful Language.
Accessibility
If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.