What do differences between own, admired, and attributed choices have to do with group induced shifts in choice
dc.contributor.author | Burnstein, Eugene | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | Vinokur, Amiram D. | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | Pichevin, Marie-France | en_US |
dc.date.accessioned | 2006-04-07T16:44:43Z | |
dc.date.available | 2006-04-07T16:44:43Z | |
dc.date.issued | 1974-09 | en_US |
dc.identifier.citation | Burnstein, Eugene, Vinokur, Amiram, Pichevin, Marie-France (1974/09)."What do differences between own, admired, and attributed choices have to do with group induced shifts in choice." Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 10(5): 428-443. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/22288> | en_US |
dc.identifier.uri | http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WJB-4D62JBP-DW/2/e0faa314f1132dc3454706994cbaf68d | en_US |
dc.identifier.uri | https://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/22288 | |
dc.description.abstract | Two studies examined certain discrepancies which have been considered important evidence in support of interpersonal comparison (value-adherence) explanations of group induced shifts in choice. These are (a) the differences between a person's own choice and the choice he predicts others would make and (b) the difference between the former and the choice he admires. Findings from the first study indicate that own choices are more extreme than those a person predicts others would make because he is more certain and confident about the former than the latter, not because he wishes to appear to outdo others as interpersonal comparison theories of choice-shift effect would have it. The second study strongly suggests that extreme choices are admired not because they display maximal adherence to a social ideal but because they imply that the person's solution to a problem involving choice is well-founded, that he has persuasive reasons for the choice. On the whole the evidence bodes well for explanations of choice-shift effects based on persuasive argumentation and poorly for those relying on interpersonal comparison processes. | en_US |
dc.format.extent | 1086676 bytes | |
dc.format.extent | 3118 bytes | |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | |
dc.format.mimetype | text/plain | |
dc.language.iso | en_US | |
dc.publisher | Elsevier | en_US |
dc.title | What do differences between own, admired, and attributed choices have to do with group induced shifts in choice | en_US |
dc.type | Article | en_US |
dc.rights.robots | IndexNoFollow | en_US |
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevel | Psychology | en_US |
dc.subject.hlbtoplevel | Social Sciences | en_US |
dc.description.peerreviewed | Peer Reviewed | en_US |
dc.contributor.affiliationum | University of Michigan, USA; Université de Provence, France | en_US |
dc.contributor.affiliationum | University of Michigan, USA; Université de Provence, France | en_US |
dc.contributor.affiliationum | University of Michigan, USA; Université de Provence, France | en_US |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/22288/1/0000728.pdf | en_US |
dc.identifier.doi | http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(74)90011-0 | en_US |
dc.identifier.source | Journal of Experimental Social Psychology | en_US |
dc.owningcollname | Interdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed |
Files in this item
Remediation of Harmful Language
The University of Michigan Library aims to describe its collections in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in them. We encourage you to Contact Us anonymously if you encounter harmful or problematic language in catalog records or finding aids. More information about our policies and practices is available at Remediation of Harmful Language.
Accessibility
If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.