Show simple item record

Pragmatic reasoning schemas

dc.contributor.authorCheng, Patricia W.en_US
dc.contributor.authorHolyoak, Keith J.en_US
dc.date.accessioned2006-04-07T18:57:57Z
dc.date.available2006-04-07T18:57:57Z
dc.date.issued1985-10en_US
dc.identifier.citationCheng, Patricia W., Holyoak, Keith J. (1985/10)."Pragmatic reasoning schemas." Cognitive Psychology 17(4): 391-416. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/25556>en_US
dc.identifier.urihttp://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WCR-4D5XCBM-4Y/2/9e37053d61997ad96ff58062644d4639en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/25556
dc.identifier.urihttp://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/sites/entrez?cmd=retrieve&db=pubmed&list_uids=4064622&dopt=citationen_US
dc.description.abstractWe propose that people typically reason about realistic situations using neither content-free syntactic inference rules nor representations of specific experiences. Rather, people reason using knowledge structures that we term pragmatic reasoning schemas, which are generalized sets of rules defined in relation to classes of goals. Three experiments examined the impact of a "permission schema" on deductive reasoning. Experiment 1 demonstrated that by evoking the permission schema it is possible to facilitate performance in Wason's selection paradigm for subjects who have had no experience with the specific content of the problems. Experiment 2 showed that a selection problem worded in terms of an abstract permission elicited better performance than one worded in terms of a concrete but arbitrary situation, providing evidence for an abstract permission schema that is free of domain-specific content. Experiment 3 provided evidence that evocation of a permission schema affects not only tasks requiring procedural knowledge, but also a linguistic rephrasing task requiring declarative knowledge. In particular, statements in the form if p then q were rephrased into the form p only if q with greater frequency for permission than for arbitrary statements, and rephrasings of permission statements produced a pattern of introduction of modals (must, can) totally unlike that observed for arbitrary conditional statements. Other pragmatic schemas, such as "causal" and "evidence" schemas can account for both linguistic and reasoning phenomena that alternative hypotheses fail to explain.en_US
dc.format.extent1871439 bytes
dc.format.extent3118 bytes
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.format.mimetypetext/plain
dc.language.isoen_US
dc.publisherElsevieren_US
dc.titlePragmatic reasoning schemasen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollowen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPsychologyen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumUniversity of Michigan, USAen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherCarnegie-Mellon University, USAen_US
dc.identifier.pmid4064622en_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/25556/1/0000098.pdfen_US
dc.identifier.doihttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(85)90014-3en_US
dc.identifier.sourceCognitive Psychologyen_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.