The Organization of Mental Verbs and Folk Theories of Knowing
dc.contributor.author | Schwanenflugel, P. J. | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | Fabricius, William | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | Noyes, C. R. | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | Bigler, K. D. | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | Alexander, J. M. | en_US |
dc.date.accessioned | 2006-04-10T18:07:12Z | |
dc.date.available | 2006-04-10T18:07:12Z | |
dc.date.issued | 1994-06 | en_US |
dc.identifier.citation | Schwanenflugel P. J., , Fabricius W. V., , Noyes C. R., , Bigler K. D., , Alexander J. M., (1994/06)."The Organization of Mental Verbs and Folk Theories of Knowing." Journal of Memory and Language 33(3): 376-395. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/31548> | en_US |
dc.identifier.uri | http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WK4-45P09DJ-Y/2/c5c040cfe9671515d2d3da081fc9685c | en_US |
dc.identifier.uri | https://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/31548 | |
dc.description.abstract | Folk theories of knowing in North American adults were studied by examining the organization of mental verbs in two tasks: (a) a Similarity Judgment Task in which subjects rated the similarity of verb pairs in terms of the way the mind is used, and (b) a Verb Extension Task in which subjects identified the mental verbs applicable to a variety of scenarios requiring specific mental activities. Organizational structure was assessed using multidimensional scaling and additive similarity tree analyses. An Attribute Rating Task was used to describe the characteristics which organized the various dimensions and clusters obtained in the scaling solutions. The folk theory of mind displayed was a naive information processing model with interactive and constructive components. | en_US |
dc.format.extent | 1265576 bytes | |
dc.format.extent | 3118 bytes | |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | |
dc.format.mimetype | text/plain | |
dc.language.iso | en_US | |
dc.publisher | Elsevier | en_US |
dc.title | The Organization of Mental Verbs and Folk Theories of Knowing | en_US |
dc.type | Article | en_US |
dc.rights.robots | IndexNoFollow | en_US |
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevel | Psychology | en_US |
dc.subject.hlbtoplevel | Social Sciences | en_US |
dc.description.peerreviewed | Peer Reviewed | en_US |
dc.contributor.affiliationum | University of Michigan | en_US |
dc.contributor.affiliationother | University of Georgia | en_US |
dc.contributor.affiliationother | Arizona State University | en_US |
dc.contributor.affiliationother | University of Georgia | en_US |
dc.contributor.affiliationother | Indiana University | en_US |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/31548/1/0000471.pdf | en_US |
dc.identifier.doi | http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1018 | en_US |
dc.identifier.source | Journal of Memory and Language | en_US |
dc.owningcollname | Interdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed |
Files in this item
Remediation of Harmful Language
The University of Michigan Library aims to describe its collections in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in them. We encourage you to Contact Us anonymously if you encounter harmful or problematic language in catalog records or finding aids. More information about our policies and practices is available at Remediation of Harmful Language.
Accessibility
If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.