Show simple item record

Mothers' social regulatory language to young children in family settings

dc.contributor.authorHalle, Tamaraen_US
dc.contributor.authorShatz, Marilynen_US
dc.date.accessioned2010-04-13T19:32:28Z
dc.date.available2010-04-13T19:32:28Z
dc.date.issued1994en_US
dc.identifier.citationHalle, Tamara; Shatz, Marilyn (1994). "Mothers' social regulatory language to young children in family settings." First Language 14(40): 083-104. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/67511>en_US
dc.identifier.issn0142-7237en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/67511
dc.description.abstractWe investigate the language mothers use to regulate social conduct in family settings with their older (4- to 5-year-old) and younger (2-year-old) children. Samples of spontaneous conversations between British mothers and their children were collected over six months. Twenty-five percent of mothers' child- directed speech was social regulatory, with significantly more such language directed to younger children. Although the most common kind of regulatory language directed to both children was imperative mands used prescriptively, our data also suggest permission statements to younger children were used often to restrict activity, whereas permission statements to older children were used largely to permit activity. We discuss results with regard to maternal sensitivity to children's developmental differences, conversational constraints in family settings, and possible cultural influences on speech styles.en_US
dc.format.extent3108 bytes
dc.format.extent1190089 bytes
dc.format.mimetypetext/plain
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.publisherSage Publicationsen_US
dc.titleMothers' social regulatory language to young children in family settingsen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelEducationen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelLinguisticsen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelHumanitiesen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumUniversity of Michiganen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumUniversity of Michiganen_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/67511/2/10.1177_014272379401404006.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1177/014272379401404006en_US
dc.identifier.sourceFirst Languageen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAndersen, E.S. (1977). Young children's knowledge of role-related speech differences: a mommy is not a daddy is not a baby Papers and Reports in Child Language Development, 13, 83-90.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreference- (1978). Will you don't snore please?: directives in young children's role-play speech Papers and Reports in Child Language Development, 15, 140-150.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBarker, R.G. & Barker, L.S. (1963). Social actions in the behavior streams of American and English children In R. G. Barker (ed.), The Stream of Behavior (New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts), 127-159.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBellinger, D. (1979). Changes in the explicitness of mothers' directives as children age Journal of Child Language, 6, 443-458.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreference- (1980). Consistency in the pattern of change in mothers' speech: some discriminant analyses Journal of Child Language, 7, 469-487.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBrown, P. & Levinson, S.C. (1978). Universals in language usage: politeness phenomena In E. N. Goody (ed.), Questions and Politeness Strategies in Social Interaction (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 56-324.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBryant, B. & Crockenberg, S. (1980). Correlates and dimensions of prosocial behavior: a study of female siblings with their mothers Child Development, 51, 529-544.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceClancy, P.M. (1986). The acquisition of communicative style in Japan In B. B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (eds), Language Socialization Across Cultures (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 213 - 250.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCorsaro, W.A. (1979). Young children's conception of status and role Sociology of Education, 52, 46-59.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDevereux, E.C., Bronfenbrenner, U. & Rodgers, R.R. (1969). Child-rearing in England and the United States: a cross-national comparison Journal of Marriage and the Family, 31, 257-270.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDunn, J. (1988). The Beginnings of Social Understanding (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDunn, J. & Brown, J. (1991). Becoming American or English? Talking about the social world in England and the United States In M. H. Bornstem (ed.), Cultural Approaches to Parenting (Hillsdale, N J : Lawrence Erlbaum), 155-172.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDunn, J., & Munn, P. (1985). Becoming a family member: family conflict and the development of social understanding in the second year Child Development, 56, 480-492.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDunn, J.F., Plomin, R. & Daniels, D. (1986). Consistency and change in mothers' behavior toward young siblings Child Development, 57, 348-356.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDunn, J. & Shatz, M. (1989). Becoming a conversationalist despite (or because of) having a sibling Child Development, 60, 399-410.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEisenberg, A.R. & Garvey, C. (1981). Children's use of verbal strategies in resolving conflicts Discourse Processes, 4, 149-170.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGarvey, C. (1975). Requests and responses in children's speech Journal of Child Language, 2, 41-63.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGearhart, M. (1983). Social plans and social episodes: the development of collaboration in role play Unpublished doctoral dissertation, City University of New York.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJacobs, B.S. & Moss, H.A. (1976). Birth order and sex of sibling as determinants of mother-infant interaction Child Development, 47, 315-322.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJones, C.P. & Adamson, L.B. (1987). Language use in mother-child and mother-child-sibling interactions Child Development, 58, 356-366.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMatsumori, A. (1981). Hahoya no kodomo e no gengo ni yoru koodoo kisei - yookyuu hyoogen no nichiber hikaku In F. C. Peng (ed.), Gengo Shuutoku no Shosoo [Aspects of language acquisition] (Hiroshima: Bunka Hyoron), 320-339.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMc Grew, W.C. (1972). An Ethological Study of Children's Behavior (New York: Academic Press).en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMunn, P.A. (1990). The development of understanding of social rules between the ages of 24 and 36 months Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cambridge, England.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNewman, D. (1978). Ownership and permission among nursery school children In J. Glick & K. A. Clarke-Stewart (eds), The Development of Social Understanding (New York : Gardner), 213-249.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceOlson, D.R. (1980). Some social aspects of meaning in oral and written language In D. Olson (ed.), The Social Foundations of Language (New York: Norton), 90-101.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRao, C.R. (1973). Linear Statistical Inference and its Applications Second edition. (New York : Wiley).en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRegistra General (1971). Census 1971. Great Britain Summary Tables (1% Sample) (London : Her Majesty's Stationery Office).en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceScarr, S. & Mc Cartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environment: a theory of genotype-environment correlations Child Development, 54, 424-435.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceShatz, M. (1978a). Children's comprehension of their mothers' question-directives Journal of Child Language, 5, 39-46.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreference- (1978b). On the development of communicative understandings: an early strategy for interpreting and respondmg to messages, Cognitive Psychology, 10, 271-301.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreference- (1991). Using cross-cultural research to inform us about the role of language in development: comparisons of Japanese, Korean, and English, and of German, American and British English In M. H. Bomstein (ed.), Cultural Approaches to Parenting (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 139-154.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreference- (1994). A Toddler's Life. Becoming a Person (New York: Oxford University Press).en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceShatz, M., Grimm, H., Wilcox, S. & Niemeier-Wind, K. (1989). The uses of modal expressions in conversations between German and American mothers and their two-year-olds Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City, April 1989.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSnow, C.E. (1972). Mothers' speech to children learning language Child Development, 43, 549-565.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStrayer, F.F. & Strayer, J. (1980). Preschool conflict and the assessment of social dominance In D. R. Omark, F. F. Strayer & D. G. Freedman (eds), Dominance Relations: An Ethological View of Human Conflict and Social Interaction (New York: Gardner), 137-158.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceVaughn, B.E. & Waters, E. (1981). Attention structure, sociometric status, and dominance: interrelations, behavioral correlates, and relationships to social competence Developmental Psychology, 17, 275-288.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.