Suspicion and Dispositional Inference
dc.contributor.author | Hilton, James L. | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | Fein, Steven | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | Miller, Dale | en_US |
dc.date.accessioned | 2010-04-14T13:38:46Z | |
dc.date.available | 2010-04-14T13:38:46Z | |
dc.date.issued | 1993 | en_US |
dc.identifier.citation | Hilton, James; Fein, Steven; Miller, Dale (1993). "Suspicion and Dispositional Inference." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 19(5): 501-512. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/68435> | en_US |
dc.identifier.issn | 0146-1672 | en_US |
dc.identifier.uri | https://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/68435 | |
dc.description.abstract | The role of suspicion in the dispositional inference process is examined. Perceivers who are led to become suspicious of the motives underlying a target's behavior appear to engage in more active and thoughtful attributional analyses than nonsuspicious perceivers. Suspicious perceivers resist drawing inferences from a target's behavior that reflect the correspondence bias (or fundamental attribution error), and they consciously deliberate about questions of plausible causes and categorizations of the target's behavior They are, however, quite willing to make strong correspondent inferences about the target if they learn additional contextual information that renders alternative explanations for the target's behavior less plausible. Implications of these findings for current multiple-stage models of the dispositional inference process are discussed, and the need for these and other models to give more consideration to the social nature of social perception is asserted. | en_US |
dc.format.extent | 3108 bytes | |
dc.format.extent | 2391059 bytes | |
dc.format.mimetype | text/plain | |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | |
dc.publisher | Sage Publications | en_US |
dc.title | Suspicion and Dispositional Inference | en_US |
dc.type | Article | en_US |
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevel | Psychology | en_US |
dc.subject.hlbtoplevel | Social Sciences | en_US |
dc.description.peerreviewed | Peer Reviewed | en_US |
dc.contributor.affiliationum | University of Michigan | en_US |
dc.contributor.affiliationother | Williams College | en_US |
dc.contributor.affiliationother | Princeton University | en_US |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/68435/2/10.1177_0146167293195003.pdf | |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1177/0146167293195003 | en_US |
dc.identifier.source | Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Anderson, N. H. (1974). Information integration: A brief survey. In D. H. Krantz, R. C. Atkinson, R. D. Luce, & P. Suppes (Eds.), Contemporary developments in mathematical psychology (pp. 236-305). San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74, 183-200. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fein, S. (1991). The suspicious mind. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fein, S., Hilton, J. L., & Miller, D. T. (1989, May). The effects of suspicion on dispositional inference& Paper presented at the Sixty-First Annual Meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fein, S., Hilton, J. L., & Miller, D. T. (1990). Suspicion of ulterior motivation and the correspondence bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 753-764. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 203-210. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gilbert, D. T. (1989). Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components of the social inference process. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 189-210). New York: Guilford. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gilbert, D. T., & Jones, E. E. (1986). Perceiver-induced constraint Interpretations of self-generated reality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 269-280. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gilbert, D. T., Mc Nulty, S. E., Giuliano, T. A., & Benson, J. E. (1992). Blurry words and fuzzy deeds: The attribution of outcome behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 18-25. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busyness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 733-740. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Hilton, J. L., Miller, D. T., Fein, S., & Darley, J. M. (1990). When dispositional inferences are suspended: Diagnosing and calibrating traits. Review International de Psychologie Sociale, 4, 519-537. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Jones, E. E. (1964). Ingratiation: A social psychological analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Jones, E. E. (1979). The rocky road from acts to dispositions. American Psychologist, 34, 107-117. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 220-265). New York: Academic Press. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Jones, E. E., Davis, K. E., & Gergen, K. (1961). Role playing variations and their informational value for person perception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 302-310. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 1-24. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 192-238). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kelley, H. H. (1972). Causal schemata and the attribution process. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 151-174). Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 107-128. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Miller, A. G., Schmidt, D., Meyer, C., & Colella, A. (1984). The perceived value of constrained behavior: Pressures toward biased inference in the attitude attribution paradigm. Social Psychology Quarterly, 47, 160-171. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Miller, D. T., Turnbull, W., & Mc Farland, C. (1989). When a coincidence is suspicious: The role of mental simulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 581-589. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Pruitt, D. G. (1968). Reciprocity and credit building in a laboratory dyad. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 143-147. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Quattrone, G. A. (1982). Overattribution and unit formation: When behavior engulfs the person. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 593-607. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Reeder, G. D., & Brewer, M. B. (1979). A schematic model of dispositional attribution in interpersonal perception. Psychological Review, 86, 61-79. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Reeder, G. D., & Fulks, J. L. (1980). When actions speak louder than words: Implicational schemata and the attribution of ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 33-46. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Reeder, G. D., Messick, D. M., & Van Avermaet, E. (1977). Dimensional asymmetry in attributional inference. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 46-57. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advance in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 173-220). New York: Academic Press. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ross, L., Amabile, T. M., & Steinmetz, J. L. (1977). Social roles, social control, and biases in social perception processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 485-494. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rothbart, M., & Park, B. (1986). On the confirmability and disconfirmability of trait concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 131-142. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Snyder, M., & Jones, E. E. (1974). Attitude attribution when behavior is constrained. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 585-600. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Thibaut, J. W., & Riecken, H. W. (1955). Some determinants and consequences of the perception of social causality. Journal of Personality, 24, 113-133. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Trope, Y. (1986). Identification and inferential processes in dispositional attribution. Psychological Review, 93, 239-257. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Trope, Y. (1989). Levels of inference in dispositional judgment. Social Cognition, 7, 296-314. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Trope, Y., Cohen, O., & Maoz, Y. (1988). The perceptual and inferential effects of situational inducements on dispositional attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 165-177. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 3-25. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Weiner, B. (1986). An attributonal theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag. | en_US |
dc.owningcollname | Interdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed |
Files in this item
Remediation of Harmful Language
The University of Michigan Library aims to describe its collections in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in them. We encourage you to Contact Us anonymously if you encounter harmful or problematic language in catalog records or finding aids. More information about our policies and practices is available at Remediation of Harmful Language.
Accessibility
If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.