Personality and Cognitive Processing of Affective Information
dc.contributor.author | Rusting, Cheryl L. | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | Larsen, Randy J. | en_US |
dc.date.accessioned | 2010-04-14T13:49:27Z | |
dc.date.available | 2010-04-14T13:49:27Z | |
dc.date.issued | 1998 | en_US |
dc.identifier.citation | Rusting, Cheryl; Larsen, Randy (1998). "Personality and Cognitive Processing of Affective Information." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 24(2): 200-213. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/68615> | en_US |
dc.identifier.issn | 0146-1672 | en_US |
dc.identifier.uri | https://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/68615 | |
dc.description.abstract | Several studies have demonstrated that the personality traits of extraversion and neuroticism are positively correlated with susceptibilities to positive and negative affect, respectively. These findings are often explained in terms of Jeffrey Gray's theory of personality, which predicts that extraverts and neurotics are differentially susceptible to stimuli that generate positive and negative emotional states. The current research provides a further test of Gray's theory using a series of cognitive tasks with positive and negative stimuli. In Study 1, participants completed a word-fragment completion task, a reaction-time task and a recall task. Results showed that extraversion was generally related to performance when stimuli were positive but not when stimuli were negative or neutral. Study 2 replicated these findings and demonstrated that the relationships between personality and performance were not mediated by current mood state. Discussion focuses on integrating a cognitive analysis of personality with existing biological theories. | en_US |
dc.format.extent | 3108 bytes | |
dc.format.extent | 2570356 bytes | |
dc.format.mimetype | text/plain | |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | |
dc.publisher | Sage Publications | en_US |
dc.title | Personality and Cognitive Processing of Affective Information | en_US |
dc.type | Article | en_US |
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevel | Psychology | en_US |
dc.subject.hlbtoplevel | Social Sciences | en_US |
dc.description.peerreviewed | Peer Reviewed | en_US |
dc.contributor.affiliationum | University of Michigan, crusting@acsu.buffalo.edu. | en_US |
dc.contributor.affiliationum | University of Michigan | en_US |
dc.description.bitstreamurl | http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/68615/2/10.1177_0146167298242008.pdf | |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1177/0146167298242008 | en_US |
dc.identifier.source | Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229-246. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129-148. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bower, G. H. (1987). Commentary on mood and memory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25, 443-456. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bradley, B. P., & Mogg, K. (1994). Mood and personality in recall of positive and negative information. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 137-141. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Bradley, B., Mogg, K., Galbraith, M., & Perrett, A. (1993). Negative recall bias and neuroticism: State vs. trait effects. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 125-127. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Clark, D. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (1985). Constraints on the effects of mood on memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1595-1608. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Costa, P. T., & Mc Crae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668-678. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Desrosiers, G., & Robinson, D. (1992). Memory and hedonic tone: "Personality" or "mood" congruence?Psychological Medicine, 22, 117-129. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Emmons, R. A., & Diener, E. (1985). Personality correlates of subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 89-97. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S.B.G. (1972). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Fazio, R. H. (1990). A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 74-97. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck's theory of personality. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for personality (pp. 246-276). New York: Springer. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1992). Promises and problems with the circumplex model of emotion. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 25-59. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1989). Extraversion, neuroticism, and susceptibility to positive and negative mood induction procedures. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 1221-1228. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132-140. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Lishman, W. A. (1974). The speed of recall of pleasant and unpleasant experiences. Psychological Medicine, 4, 212-218. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Martin, M., Ward, J. C., & Clark, D. M. (1983). Neuroticism and the recall of positive and negative personality information. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, 495-503. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mayer, J. D., & Volanth, A. J. (1985). Cognitive involvement in the mood response system. Motivation and Emotion, 9, 261-275. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mayo, P. R. (1983). Personality traits and the retrieval of positive and negative memories. Personality and Individual Difference-s, 4, 465-471. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mayo, P. R. (1989). A further study of the personality-congruent recall effect. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 247-252. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Mc Fatter, R. M. (1994). Interactions in predicting mood from extraversion and neuroticism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 570-578. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Meyer, G. J., & Shack, J. R. (1989). The structural convergence of mood and personality: Evidence for old and new "directions."Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 697-706. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Okun, M. A., Stock, W. A., Snead, L., & Wiermaa, P. (1987). Neuroticism and autobiographical memory for positive and negative events. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 965-967. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | O'Malley, M. N., & Gillette, C. S. (1984). Exploring the relations between traits and emotions. Journal of Personality, 52, 274-284. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Parrott, W. G., & Sabini, J. (1990). Mood and memory under natural conditions: Evidence for mood incongruent recall. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 321-336. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Ruiz-Caballero, J. A., & Gonzalez, P. (1995). Neuroticism, mood, and retrieval of negative personal memories. Journal of General Psychology, 122, 29-35. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Rusting, C. L., & Larsen, R. J. (1995). Moods as sources of stimulation: Relationships between personality and desired mood states. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 321-329. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1988). Mood and memory: Evaluating the network theory of affect. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 211-251. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Smith, S. M., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Personality moderators of mood congruency effects on cognition: The role of self-esteem and negative mood regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1092-1107. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Teasdale, J. D., & Russell, M. L. (1983). Differential effects of induced mood on the recall of positive, negative and neutral words. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 2Z163-171. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wallace, J. F., & Newman, J. P. (1990). Differential effects of reward and punishment cues on response speed in anxious and impulsive individuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 999-1009. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Wallace, J. F., Newman, J. P., & Bachorowski, J. (1991). Failures of response modulation: Impulsive behavior in anxious and impulsive individuals. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 23-44. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). On traits and temperament: General and specific factors of emotional experience and their relation to the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 60, 441-476. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-1070. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Williams, J.M.G., Watts, F. N., Mc Leod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. Chichester, UK:Wiley. | en_US |
dc.identifier.citedreference | Young, G.C.D., & Martin, M. (1981). Processing of information about self by neurotics. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 20, 205-212. | en_US |
dc.owningcollname | Interdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed |
Files in this item
Remediation of Harmful Language
The University of Michigan Library aims to describe its collections in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in them. We encourage you to Contact Us anonymously if you encounter harmful or problematic language in catalog records or finding aids. More information about our policies and practices is available at Remediation of Harmful Language.
Accessibility
If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.