Show simple item record

Misanthropic Person Memory when the Need to Self-Enhance is Absent

dc.contributor.authorYbarra, Oscaren_US
dc.date.accessioned2010-04-14T13:53:17Z
dc.date.available2010-04-14T13:53:17Z
dc.date.issued1999en_US
dc.identifier.citationYbarra, Oscar (1999). "Misanthropic Person Memory when the Need to Self-Enhance is Absent." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 25(2): 261-269. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/68679>en_US
dc.identifier.issn0146-1672en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/68679
dc.description.abstractThis research examined the role that the removal of the need or ability to self-enhance can play in the misanthropic processing of attributed behavioral information (i.e., remembering best negative, internally attributed behaviors and positive externally attributed behaviors). Experiment 1demonstrated that removing a person’s need to self-enhance by increasing his or her self-esteem eliminated misanthropic memory, whereas misanthropy was preserved for control participants and perceivers who had experienced a decrease in self-esteem. Furthermore, controlling for participants’ self-evaluations eliminated the memory pattern differences between the two experimental conditions. Experiment 2 demonstrated that canceling the ability to self-enhance by having perceivers form an impression of themselves eliminated the misanthropy effect. However, the misanthropy effect was replicated when perceivers learned about an unknown other. The results were discussed with regard to the situations and factors that can increase or reduce the need to self-enhance and their implications for social information processing.en_US
dc.format.extent3108 bytes
dc.format.extent2157713 bytes
dc.format.mimetypetext/plain
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.publisherSage Publicationsen_US
dc.titleMisanthropic Person Memory when the Need to Self-Enhance is Absenten_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPsychologyen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumUniversity of Michiganen_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/68679/2/10.1177_0146167299025002011.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1177/0146167299025002011en_US
dc.identifier.sourcePersonality and Social Psychology Bulletinen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAnderson, N. H. (1965). Primary effects in personality impression formation using a generalized order effect paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 1-9.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBaumeister, R. F. (1994). Self-regulation and ego threat: Motivated cognition, self-deception, and destructive goal setting. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action. New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBaumeister, R. F., & Cairns, K. J. (1992). Repression and self-presentation: When audiences interfere with self-deceptive strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 851-862.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBell, L. G., Wicklund, R. A., Manko, G., & Larkin, C. (1976). When unexpected behaviors is attributed to the environment. Journal of Research in Personality, 10, 316-327.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBoski, P. (1983). A study of person-perception in Nigeria: Ethnicity and self versus other attributions for achievement-related outcomes. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 14, 85-108.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBower, G. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129-145.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceClore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In R. S. Wyer and T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition: Vol. 1. Basic processes. Hillsdale. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCrocker, J. (1993). Memory for information about others: Effects of self-esteem and performance feedback. Journal of Research in Personality, 27, 35-48.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDutta, S., Kanungo, R. N., & Freibergs, V. (1972). Retention of affective material: Effects of intensity of affect on retrieval. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 64-80.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFein, S., & Spencer, S. (1997). Prejudice as self-image maintenance: Affirming the self through derogating others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 31-44.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFiedler, K. (1988). Emotional mood, cognitive style, and behavior regulation. In K. Fiedler & J. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and social behavior (pp. 100-119). Toronto, Canada: Hogrefe International.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFiske, S. T. (1980). Attention and weight in person perception: The impact of negative and extreme behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 889-906.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceForgas, J. P., & Bower, G. H. (1987). Mood effects on person-perception judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 53-60.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFry, P. S., & Ghosh, R. (1980). Attributions of success and failure: Comparison of cultural differences between Asian and Caucasian children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 11, 343-363.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHamilton, D. L., Dugan, P. M., & Trolier, T. K. (1985). The formation of stereotypic beliefs: Further evidence for distinctiveness-based illusory correlations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 5-17.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHamilton, D. L., & Zanna, M. (1972). Differential weighting of favorable and unfavorable attributes in impressions of personality. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 6, 204-212.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHeine, S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1997). The cultural construction of self-enhancement: An examination of group-serving biases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1268-1283.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHewstone, M. (1990). The fiultimate attribution errorfl? A review of the literature on intergroup causal attribution. European Journal of Social Psychology, 20, 311-335.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHoward, J. W., & Rothbart, M. (1980). Social categorization and memory for in-group and out-group behavior. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 38, 301-310.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKashima, Y., & Triandis, H. C. (1986). The self-serving bias in attribution as a coping strategy: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 17, 83-97.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKlein, J. G. (1991). Negativity effects in impression formation: A test in the political arena. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 412-418.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKuiper, N. A., & Mac Donald, M. R. (1982). Self and other perception in mild depressives. Social Cognition, 1, 233-239.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMackie, D. M., Hamilton, D. L., Susskind, J., & Rosselli, F. (1996). Social psychological foundations of stereotype formation. In C. N. Macrae, C. Stangor, & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Stereotypes and stereotyping. New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePeeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and informational negativity effects. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology, 11, 33-60. Wiley & Sons.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePettigrew, T. F. (1979). The ultimate attribution error: Extending Allport’s cognitive analysis of prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 5, 461-476.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380-391.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSilverman, I. (1964). Self-esteem and differential responsiveness to success and failure. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 115-119.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSkowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1987). Social judgment and social memory: The role of cue diagnosticity in negativity, positivity, and extremity biases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 689-699.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSnyder, M. L., Stephan, W. G., & Rosenfield, D. (1978). Attributional egotism. In J. H. Harvey, W. Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSteele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol., 21, pp. 261-302). New York: Academic Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStephan, W. G. (1977). Stereotyping: the role of ingroup-outgroup differences in causal attribution for behavior. Journal of Social Psychology, 101, 255-266.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTajfel, H. (1978). The achievement of group differentiation. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations. London: Academic Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 33, pp. 1-39). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTesser, A., Martin, L. L., & Cornell, D. P. (1994). On the substitutability of Self-Protective Mechanisms. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action. New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWills, T. A. (1981). Downward comparison principles in social psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 245-271.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWood, J. V. (1989). Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 231-248.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWood, J. V., Taylor, S. E., & Lichtman, R. R. (1985). Social comparison in adjustment to breast cancer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1169-1183.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWyer, R. S. (1970). Information redundancy, inconsistency, and novelty and their role in impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 6, 111-127.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWyer, R. S., & Gordon, S. (1982). The recall of information about persons and groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 128-164.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceYbarra, O. (1997). Unpublished raw data, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceYbarra, O., Schaberg, L., & Keiper, S. N. (1998). Target favorability and social information processing. Manuscript submitted for publication, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceYbarra, O., & Stephan, W. G. (1996). Misanthropic person memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 691-700.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceYbarra, O., Stephan, W. G., & Schaberg, L. (1998). Person memory for the attributed behavior of group members: From misanthropy to sexism. Manuscript submitted for publication, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.