Show simple item record

Accessible Content and Accessibility Experiences: The Interplay of Declarative and Experiential Information in Judgment

dc.contributor.authorSchwarz, Norberten_US
dc.date.accessioned2010-04-14T13:53:42Z
dc.date.available2010-04-14T13:53:42Z
dc.date.issued1998en_US
dc.identifier.citationSchwarz, Norbert (1998). "Accessible Content and Accessibility Experiences: The Interplay of Declarative and Experiential Information in Judgment." Personality and Social Psychology Review 2(2): 87-99. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/68686>en_US
dc.identifier.issn1088-8683en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/68686
dc.identifier.urihttp://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/sites/entrez?cmd=retrieve&db=pubmed&list_uids=15647137&dopt=citationen_US
dc.description.abstractRecall tasks render 2 distinct sources of information available: the recalled content and the experienced ease or difficulty with which it can be brought to mind. Because retrieving many pieces of information is more difficult than retrieving only a few, reliance on accessible content and subjective accessibility experiences leads to opposite judgmental outcomes. People are likely to base judgments on accessibility experiences when they adopt a heuristic processing strategy and the informational value of the experience is not called into question. When the experience is considered nondiagnostic, or when a systematic processing strategy is adopted, people rely on accessible content. Implications for the operation of the availability heuristic and the emergence of knowledge accessibility effects are discussed.en_US
dc.format.extent3108 bytes
dc.format.extent2404396 bytes
dc.format.mimetypetext/plain
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.publisherSage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CAen_US
dc.titleAccessible Content and Accessibility Experiences: The Interplay of Declarative and Experiential Information in Judgmenten_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPsychologyen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumUniversity of Michiganen_US
dc.identifier.pmid15647137en_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/68686/2/10.1207_s15327957pspr0202_2.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1207/s15327957pspr0202_2en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBelli, R. F., Winkielman, P., Read, J. D., Schwarz, N., & Lynn, S. J. (in press). Recalling more childhood events leads to judgments of poorer memory: Implications for the recovered/false memory debate. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBenjamin, A. S., & Bjork, R. A. (1996). Retrieval fluency as a metacognitive index. In L. M. Reder (Ed.), Implicit memory and metacognition (pp. 309-338). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBiller, B., Bless, H., & Schwarz, N. (1992, April). Die Leichtigkeit der Erinnerung als Information in der Urteilsbildung: Der Einfluβ der Fragenreihenfolge [Ease of recall as information: The impact of question order]. Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen, OsnabrÜck, Germany.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBodenhausen, G. V., & Wyer, R. S. (1987). Social cognition and social reality: Information acquisition and use in the laboratory and the real world. In H. J. Hippler, N. Schwarz, & S. Sudman (Eds.), Social information processing and survey methodology (pp. 6-41). New York: Springer-Verlag.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBrewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Eds.), Advances in Social Cognition, (Vol. 1, pp. 1-36). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 215-252). New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceClore, G. L. (1992). Cognitive phenomenology: Feelings and the construction of judgment. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 133-163). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEagly, A., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1-74). San Diego, CA: Academic.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGabrielcik, A., & Fazio, R. H. (1984). Priming and frequency estimation: A strict test of the availability heuristic. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 85-89.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGrayson, C. E., & Schwarz, N. (1997). Beliefs influence information processing strategies: Declarative and experiential information in risk assessment. Unpublished manuscript.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHaddock, G., Rothman, A., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Are (some) reports of attitude strength context dependent? Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 24, 313-317.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHiggins, E. T. (1989). Knowledge accessibility and activation: Subjectivity and suffering from unconscious sources. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp.75-123). New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHiggins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In E. T. Higgins & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133-168). New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHiggins, E. T., Rholes, W. S., & Jones, C. R. (1977). Category accessibility and impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 141-154.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJacoby, L. L., & Kelley, C. M. (1987). Unconscious influences of memory for a prior event. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 314-336.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJacoby, L. L., & Whitehouse, K. (1989). An illusion of memory: False recognition influenced by unconscious perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 126-135.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKelley, C. M., & Jacoby, L. L. (1996). Memory attributions: Remembering, knowing, and feeling of knowing. In L. M. Reder (Ed.), Implicit memory and metacognition (pp. 287-308). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKelley, C. M., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Remembering mistaken for knowing: Ease of retrieval as a basis for confidence in answers to general knowledge questions. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 1-24.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKelley, H. H. (1972). Causal schemata and the attribution process. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKoriat, A. (1993). How do we know that we know? The accessibility model of the feeling of knowing. Psychological Review, 100, 609-639.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLombardi, W. J., Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1987). The role of consciousness in priming effects on categorization: Assimilation versus contrast as a function of awareness of the priming task. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 411-429.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMarkus, H. R. (1977). Self-schemata and processing of information about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 63-78.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMartin, L. L. (1986). Set/reset: Use and disuse of concepts in impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 493-504.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMartin, L. L., & Achee, J. W. (1992). Beyond accessibility: The role of processing objectives in judgment. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgment (pp. 195-216). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMartin, L. L., & Clark, L. F. (1990). Social cognition: Exploring the mental processes involved in human social interaction. In M. W. Eysenck (Ed.), Cognitive psychology: An international review (pp. 265-310). Chichester, England: Wiley.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMedin, D. L., & Ross, B. H. (1997). Cognitive psychology (2nd ed.). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePayne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. New York: Cambridge University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePetty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123-205). New York: Academic.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRoss, C. A. (1989). Multiple personality disorder: Diagnosis, clinical features, and treatment. New York: Wiley.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRothman, A. J., & Hardin, C. (1997). Differential use of the availability heuristic in social judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 123-138.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRothman, A. J., & Schwarz, N. (in press). Constructing perceptions of vulnerability: Personal relevance and the use of experiential information in health judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchwarz, N. (1995). Social cognition: Information accessibility and use in social judgment. In E. E. Smith & D. N. Osherson (Eds.), Thinking. An invitation to cognitive science (Vol. 3, 2nd ed., pp. 345-376). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1992). Constructing reality and its alternatives: Assimilation and contrast effects in social judgment. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgment (pp. 217-245). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 195-202.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513-523.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1996). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: A handbook of basic principles (pp. 433-465). New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchwarz, N., & Schuman, H. (1997). Political knowledge, attribution, and inferred political interest: The operation of buffer items. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 9, 191-195.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchwarz, N., Strack, F., & Mai, H. P. (1991). Assimilation and contrast effects in part-whole question sequences: A conversational logic analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55, 3-23.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSherman, S. J., & Corty, E. (1984). Cognitive heuristics. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 189-286). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStepper, S., & Strack, F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emotional and nonemotional feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 211-220.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStrack, F. (1985). Urteilsheuristiken [Judgmental heuristics]. In D. Frey & M. Irle (Eds.), Theorien der Sozialpsychologie (Vol. 3, pp. 239-267). Bern, Switzerland: Huber.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStrack, F. (1992). The different routes to social judgment: Experiential versus informational strategies. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 249-276). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStrack, F., Schwarz, N., Bless, H., KÜbler, A., & WÄnke, M. (1993). Awareness of the influence as a determinant of assimilation versus contrast. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 53-62.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTaylor, S. E. (1982). The availability bias in social perception and interaction. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 190-200). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207-232.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceVaughn, L. A. (1997). Effects of expertise on use of recall experiences and recalled information for social judgments. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWÄnke, M., Bless, H., & Biller, B. (1996). Subjective experience versus content of information in the construction of attitude judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1105-1113.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWÄnke. M., Bohner, G., & Jurkowitsch, A. (in press). There are many reasons to drive a BMW: Does imagined ease of argument generation influence attitudes? Journal of Consumer Research.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWÄnk, M., Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1995). The availability heuristic revisited: Experienced ease of retrieval in mundane frequency estimates. Acta Psychologica, 89, 83-90.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWinkielman, P., Schwarz, N., & Belli, R. F. (1998). The role of ease of retrieval and attribution in memory judgments: Judging your memory as worse despite recalling more events. Psychological Science, 9, 46-48.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWyer, R. S., & Srull, T. K. (1989). Memory and cognition in its social context. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceZillman, D. (1978). Attribution and misattribution of excitatory reactions. In J. H. Harvey, W. Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (Vol. 2, pp. 335-368). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.