Show simple item record

Attention Allocation and Impression Formation

dc.contributor.authorHilton, James L.en_US
dc.contributor.authorKlein, Jillen_US
dc.contributor.authorvon Hippel, Williamen_US
dc.date.accessioned2010-04-14T13:56:38Z
dc.date.available2010-04-14T13:56:38Z
dc.date.issued1991en_US
dc.identifier.citationHilton, James; Klein, Jill; von Hippel, William (1991). "Attention Allocation and Impression Formation." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 17(5): 548-559. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/68735>en_US
dc.identifier.issn0146-1672en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/68735
dc.description.abstractAn experiment examined the attention allocation strategies that perceivers employ when they are confronted with expectancy-relevant information and the mediating roles that these strategies play in impression formation. A dichotic listening task was used to measure subjects' attention allocation strategies. Subjects monitored a control child in one ear, while in the other ear they heard a target child perform in a manner that was either initially consistent or inconsistent with their expectations. They then evaluated the target on a number of dimensions. Subjects who received initially inconsistent information from the target allocated more attention to him and became more cognitively complex in their final evaluations than subjects who received initially consistent information. Furthermore, the greater attention paid by these subjects led them to become more moderate in their evaluations of the target. The results are discussed in light of the person perception literature.en_US
dc.format.extent3108 bytes
dc.format.extent2173164 bytes
dc.format.mimetypetext/plain
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.publisherSage Publicationsen_US
dc.titleAttention Allocation and Impression Formationen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPsychologyen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumUniversity of Michiganen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumUniversity of Michiganen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherOhio State Universityen_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/68735/2/10.1177_0146167291175010.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1177/0146167291175010en_US
dc.identifier.sourcePersonality and Social Psychology Bulletinen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAnderson, N. H. (1965). Averaging versus adding as a stimulus combination rule in impression formation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 394-400.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAnderson, N. E. S., & Norman, A. (1964). Order effects in impression formation in four classes of stimuli. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 467-471.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBargh, J. A., & Thein, R. D. (1985). Individual construct accessibility, person memory, and the recall-judgment link: The case of information overload. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1129-1146.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBelmore, S. M. (1987). Determinants of attention during impression formation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 480-489.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBelmore, S. M., & Hubbard, M. L. (1987). The role of advance expectancies in person memory Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 61-70.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBruner, J. S. (1957). Going beyond the information given. In J. S. Bruner, E. Brunswik, L. Festinger, F. Heider, K. F. Muenzinger, C. E. Osgood, & D. Rapaport (Eds.), Contemporary approaches to cognition (pp. 41-69). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 554-559.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCohen, C. E. (1981). Person categories and social perception: Testing some boundaries of the processing effects of prior knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 441-452.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDarley, J. M., Fleming, J. H., Hilton, J. L., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (1988). Dispelling negative expectancies: The impact of interaction goals and target characteristics on the expectancy confirmation process. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 19-36.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDarley, J. M., & Goethals, G. R. (1980). People's analyses of the causes of ability-linked performance. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 1-37). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDarley, J. M., & Gross, P. H. (1983). A hypothesis-confirming bias in labeling effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 20-33.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceErber, R., & Fiske, S. T. (1984). Outcome dependency and attention to inconsistent information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 709-726.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFischoff, B., & Beyth, R. (1975). "I knew it would happen": Remembered probabilities of once-future things. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 13, 1-16.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum model of impression formation from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1-74). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1984). Social cognition. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGollwitzer, P. M., & Kinney, R. F. (1989). Effects of deliberative and implemental mind-sets on illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psycology, 56, 531-542.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHastie, R. (1980). Memory for behavioral information that confirms or contradicts a personality impression. In R. Hastie, T. M. Ostrom, E. B. Ebbesen, R. S. Wyer, D. L. Hamilton, & D. E. Carlston (Eds.), Person memory: The cognitive basis of social perception (pp. 155-177). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHastie, R., & Kumar, P. A. (1979). Person memory: Personality traits as organizing principles in memory for behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 25-38.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHastie, R., Park, B., & Weber, R. (1984). Social memory. In R S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 151-212). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHeider, F. (1958). The psychology of intepersonal relations. New York: Wiley.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHemsley, G. D., & Marmurek, H. H. C. (1982). Person memory: The processing of consistent and inconsistent person information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 433-438.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJones, E. E., & Goethals, G. R. (1971). Order effects in impression formation: Attribution context and the nature of the entity. In E. E. Jones, D. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJones, E. E., & Mc Gillis, D. (1976). Correspondent inferences and the attribution cube: A comparative reappraisal. In J. H. Harvey, W. J. Ickes, & R F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJones, E. E., Rock, L., Shaver, K. G., Goethals, G. R., & Ward, L. M. (1968). Pattern of performance and ability attribution: An unexpected primacy effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 317-340.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKulik,J. A. (1983). Confirmatory attribution and the perpetuation of social beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1171-1181.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLinville, P. W. (1982). The complexity-extremity effect and age-based stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 193-211.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLinville, P. W., & Jones, E. E. (1980). Polarized appraisals of outgroup members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 689-703.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2098-2109.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMayo, C. W., & Crockett, W. H. (1964). Cognitive complexity and primacy-recency effects in impression formation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 335-388.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMoray, N. (1969). Listening and attention. Baltimore: Penguin Books.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNeisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. New York: W. H. Freeman.-en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNidorff, L. J., & Crockett, W. H. (1965). Cognitive complexity and the integration of conflicting information in written impressions. Journal of Social Psychology, 66, 165-169.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNeuberg, S. L. (1989). The goal of forming accurate impressions during social interactions: Attenuating the impact of negative expectancies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 374-386.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNeuberg, S. L., & Fiske, S. T. (1987). Motivational influences on impression formation: Outcome dependency, accuracy-driven attention, and individuating processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 431-441.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePyszczynski, T. A., & Greenberg, J. (1981). Role of disconfirmed expectancies in the instigation of attributional processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 31-38.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRosenkrantz, P. S., & Crockett, W. H. (1965). Some factors influencing the assimilation of disparate information in impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 397-402.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceScott, W. A., Osgood, D. W., & Peterson, C. (1979). Cognitive structure: Theory and measurement of individual differences. Washington, DC: Winston.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceShapiro,J., & Hilton,J. L. (1988, May). The impact of interaction goals on expectancy confirmation. Paper presented at the 60th Annual Meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSkowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1989). Negativity and extremity biases in impression formation: A review of explanations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 131-142.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSnyder, M., & Cantor, N. (1979). Testing hypotheses about other people: The. use of historical knowledge. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 15, 330-342.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSnyder, M., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (1978). Hypothesis-testing processes in social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1202-1212.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSrull, T. K. (1981). Person memory: Some tests of associative storage and retrieval models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 440-463.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSrull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (1989). Person memory and judgment. Psychological Review, 96, 58-83.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTesser, A., & Leone, C. (1977). Cognitive schemas and thought as determinants of attitude change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 340-356.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTetlock, P. E. (1983). Accountability and complexity of thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 74-83.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTreisman, A. M. (1969). Strategies and models of selective attention. Psychological Review, 76, 282-299.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTrope, Y. (1986). Identification and inferential processes in dispositional attribution. Psychological Review, 93, 239-257.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWason, P. C. (1960). On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 129-140.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWhite,J. D., & Carlston, D. E. (1983). Consequences of schemata for attention, impressions, and recall in complex social interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 538-549.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe its collections in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in them. We encourage you to Contact Us anonymously if you encounter harmful or problematic language in catalog records or finding aids. More information about our policies and practices is available at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.