Show simple item record

How Priming the Private Self or Collective Self Affects the Relative Weights of Attitudes and Subjective Norms

dc.contributor.authorYbarra, Oscaren_US
dc.contributor.authorTrafimow, Daviden_US
dc.date.accessioned2010-04-14T14:08:40Z
dc.date.available2010-04-14T14:08:40Z
dc.date.issued1998en_US
dc.identifier.citationYbarra, Oscar; Trafimow, David (1998). "How Priming the Private Self or Collective Self Affects the Relative Weights of Attitudes and Subjective Norms." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 24(4): 362-370. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/68938>en_US
dc.identifier.issn0146-1672en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/68938
dc.description.abstractThree experiments tested the hypothesis that the accessibility of the private self and the collective self affects the relative weights given to attitudes and subjective norms when forming a behavioral intention. The results of Experiment 1 indicate that increasing the accessibility of the private self caused participants to place more weight on attitudes than subjective norms but that increasing the accessibility of the collective self caused participants to place more weight on subjective norms than on attitudes. Experiments 2 and 3, using a subtle priming procedure, replicated this pattern of results. In addition, the findings of Experiment 3 provided direct evidence for the differential priming of the two self-concepts. Hence, the data suggest an intimate connection between the accessibility of the private and collective selves and whether people use attitudes or subjective norms to make behavioral intentions.en_US
dc.format.extent3108 bytes
dc.format.extent1819550 bytes
dc.format.mimetypetext/plain
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.publisherSage Publicationsen_US
dc.titleHow Priming the Private Self or Collective Self Affects the Relative Weights of Attitudes and Subjective Normsen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPsychologyen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumUniversity of Michigan, oybarra Cumich.edu.en_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherNew Mexico State Universityen_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/68938/2/10.1177_0146167298244003.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1177/0146167298244003en_US
dc.identifier.sourcePersonality and Social Psychology Bulletinen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAjzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceArie, O. G., Durand, R. M., & Bearden, W. O. (1979). Attitudinal and normative dimensions of opinion leaders and nonleaders. The Journal of Psychology, 101, 305-312.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBargh, J. A., Bond, R. N., Lombardi, W. J., & Tota, M. E. (1986). The additive nature of chronic and temporary sources of construct accessibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 869-878.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBowman, C. H., & Fishbein, M. (1978). Understanding public reactions to energy proposals: An application of the Fishbein Model. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 8, 319-340.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. New York: Harcourt Brace jovanovich.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFishbein, M. (1967). Attitude and the prediction of behavior. In M. Fishbein (Ed.), Readings in attitude theory and measurement (pp. 477-492). New York: John Wiley.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFishbein, M. (1980). Theory of reasoned action: Some applications and implications. In H. Howe & M. Page (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 65-116). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFishbein, M., Middlestadt, S. E., & Trafimow, D. (1993). Social norms for condom use: Implications for HIV prevention interventions of a KABP survey with heterosexuals in the eastern Caribbean. Advances in Consumer Research, 20, 292-296.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFishbein, M., Trafimow, D., Francis, C., Helquist, M., Eustace, M. A., Ooms, M., & Middlestadt, S. E. (1993). AIDS knowledge, attitudes, beliefs, and practices (KABP) in two Caribbean countries: A comparative analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 23, 687-702.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFishbein, M., Trafimow, D., Middlestadt, S. E., Helquist, M., Francis, C., & Eustace, M. A. (1995). Using an AIDS KABP Survey to identify determinants of condom use among sexually active adults from St. Vincent and the Grenadines. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 1-20.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGreenwald, A. G., & Pratkanis, A. R. (1984). The self. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 129-178). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHiggins, E. T. (1989). Knowledge accessibility and activation: Subjectivity and suffering from unconscious sources. In J. S. Uleman &J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 75-123). New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHiggins, E. T., Bargh, J. A., & Lombardi, W. (1985). The nature of priming effects on categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 59-69.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJaccard, J. J., & Davidson, A. R. (1972). Toward an understanding of family planning behaviors: An initial investigation. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 2, 228-235.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLiska, A. E. (1984). A critical examination of the causal structure of the Fishbein/Ajzen attitude-behavior model. Social Psychology Quarterly, 47, 61-74.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMarkus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMarkus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41, 954-969.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMarkus, H., & Wurf, E. (1987). The dynamic self-concept. A social psychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 299-337.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMiller, L. E., & Grush, J. E. (1984). Individual differences in attitudinal versus normative determination of behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22190-202.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMiniard, P. W., & Cohen, J. B. (1981). An examination of the Fishbein-Ajzen behavioral-intentions model's concepts and measures. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 309-329.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceOliver, R. L., & Bearden, W. O. (1985). Crossover effects in the theory of reasoned action: A moderating influence attempt. Journal of Consumer Research, 12, 324-340.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceShimp, T. A., & Kavas, A. (1984). The theory of reasoned action applied to coupon usage. Journal of Consumer Research, 11, 795-809.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSingelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-concepts. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 580-591.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStasson, M., & Fishbein, M. (1990). The relation between perceived risk and preventive action: Within-subject analysis of perceived driving risk and intentions to wear seat belts. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 20, 1541-1547.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTrafimow, D. (1994). Predicting intentions to use a condom from perceptions of normative pressure and confidence in those perceptions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 2151-2163.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTrafimow, D. (in press). A theory of attitudes, subjective norms, and private versus collective self-concepts. In D. J. Terry & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Attitudes, behaviors, and social context: The role of norms and group membership. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTrafimow, D., & Finlay, K. (1996). The importance of subjective norms for a minority of people: Between-participants and within-participants analyses. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 820-828.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTrafimow, D., & Fishbein, M. (1994a). The importance of risk in determining the extent to which attitudes affect intentions to wear seat belts. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 1-11.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTrafimow, D., & Fishbein, M. (1994b). The moderating effect of behavior type on the subjective norm-behavior relationship. Journal of Social Psychology, 134, 755-763.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTrafimow, D., & Fishbein, M. (1995). Do people really distinguish between attitudinal and normative beliefs? British Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 257-266.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTrafimow, D., Silverman, E. S., Fan, R. M., & Law, J.S.F. (1997). The effects of language and priming on the relative accessibility of the private self and the collective self. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28, 107-123.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTrafimow, D., Triandis, H. C., & Goto, S. G. (1991). Some tests of the distinction between the private self and the collective self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 649-655.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTriandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506-520.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTriandis, H. C. (1994). Culture and social behavior. New York: Mc Graw-Hill.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceVan de Putte, B. (1991). 20 years of the theory of reasoned action of Fishbein and Ajzen: A meta-analysis. Unpublished manuscript, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceYbarra, O., & Trafimow, D. (1997). Unpublished raw data, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.