Show simple item record

Predicting Individual Evaluations from Group Evaluations and Vice Versa

dc.contributor.authorKunda, Zivaen_US
dc.contributor.authorNisbett, Richard E.en_US
dc.date.accessioned2010-04-14T14:19:39Z
dc.date.available2010-04-14T14:19:39Z
dc.date.issued1988en_US
dc.identifier.citationKunda, Ziva; Nisbett, Richard (1988). "Predicting Individual Evaluations from Group Evaluations and Vice Versa." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 14(2): 326-334. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/69123>en_US
dc.identifier.issn0146-1672en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/69123
dc.description.abstractWe examined people's beliefs about how well an individual's evaluations can predict the average evaluations of a group and how well a group's average evaluations can predict those of an individual. The individual in question was either the self or a stranger. Subjects believed that the group predicts a stranger better than the stranger predicts the group, but believed that the self predicts the group as well as or better than the group predicts the self This asymmetry in estimates of predictability mirrors asymmetries found by other researchers for similarity judgments, suggesting that beliefs about predictability may be guided by similarity judgments. This may lead to errors in predictions about self and others, including the false consensus bias.en_US
dc.format.extent3108 bytes
dc.format.extent818983 bytes
dc.format.mimetypetext/plain
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.publisherSage Publicationsen_US
dc.titlePredicting Individual Evaluations from Group Evaluations and Vice Versaen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPsychologyen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumUniversity of Michiganen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherPrinceton Universityen_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/69123/2/10.1177_0146167288142010.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1177/0146167288142010en_US
dc.identifier.sourcePersonality and Social Psychology Bulletinen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFischhoff, B., Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1979). Subjective sensitivity analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 23, 339-359.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFong, G. T., Krantz, D. H., & Nisbett, R. E. (1986). The effects of statistical training on thinking about everyday problems. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 253-292.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHolyoak, K. J., & Gordon, P. C. (1979). Social reference points. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 881-887.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of repre-sentativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430-454.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80, 237-251.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKendall, M. G. (1962). Rank correlation methods. London: Charles Griffin.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKlein, S. B., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1986). Elaboration, organization, and the self-reference effect in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 26-38.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKunda, Z., & Nisbett, R. E. (1986a). The psychometrics of everyday life. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 195-224.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKunda, Z., & Nisbett, R. E. (1986b). Prediction and the partial understanding of the law of large numbers. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 339-354.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNisbett, R. E., Krantz, D. H., Jepson, C., & Kunda, Z. (1983). The use of statistical heuristics in everyday inductive reasoning. Psychological Review, 90, 339-363.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNisbett, R. E., & Kunda, Z. (1985). The perception of social distributions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 297-311.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNisbett, R. E., & Ross L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRoss, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The false consensus phenomenon: An attributional bias in self-perception and social perception processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 279-301.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSrull, T. K., & Gaelick, L. (1983). General principles and individual differences in the self as habitual reference point: An examination of the self-other judgments of similarity. Social Cognition, 2, 108-121.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTversky, A. (1977). Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 84, 327-352.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1980). Causal schemas in judgment about uncertainty. In M. Fishbein (Ed.), Progress in social psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.