Show simple item record

Construction Learning as a Function of Frequency, Frequency Distribution, and Function

dc.contributor.authorEllis, Nick C.en_US
dc.contributor.authorFerreira–junior, Fernandoen_US
dc.date.accessioned2010-06-01T20:21:20Z
dc.date.available2010-06-01T20:21:20Z
dc.date.issued2009-09en_US
dc.identifier.citationELLIS, NICK C.; FERREIRA–JUNIOR, FERNANDO (2009). "Construction Learning as a Function of Frequency, Frequency Distribution, and Function." The Modern Language Journal 93(3): 370-385. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/73472>en_US
dc.identifier.issn0026-7902en_US
dc.identifier.issn1540-4781en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/73472
dc.format.extent478604 bytes
dc.format.extent3109 bytes
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.format.mimetypetext/plain
dc.publisherBlackwell Publishing Incen_US
dc.rights© 2009 by The Modern Language Journalen_US
dc.titleConstruction Learning as a Function of Frequency, Frequency Distribution, and Functionen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelLinguisticsen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelHumanitiesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherFederal University of Minas Gerais Av. Antonio Carlos 6627, Pampulha UFMG/FALE/PosLin, Sala 4039 Belo Horizonte, MG, CEP 31270-901 Brazil Email: fernandoufmg@gmail.comen_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/73472/1/j.1540-4781.2009.00896.x.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/j.1540-4781.2009.00896.xen_US
dc.identifier.sourceThe Modern Language Journalen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAllan, L. G. ( 1980 ). A note on measurement of contingency between two binary variables in judgment tasks. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 15, 147 – 149.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBates, E., & MacWhinney, B. ( 1987 ). Competition, variation, and language learning. In B. MacWhinney ( Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition ( pp. 157 – 193 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChristiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. ( 2008 ). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 31, 489 – 509.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceClark, E. V. ( 1978 ). Discovering what words can do. In D. Farkas, W. M. Jacobsen, & K. W. Todrys ( Eds.), Papers from the parasession on the lexicon, Chicago Linguistics Society April 14 – 15, 1978 ( pp. 34 – 57 ). Chicago: Chicago Linguistics Society.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCohen, H., & Lefebvre, C. ( Eds.). ( 2005 ). Handbook of categorization in cognitive science. Mahwah, NJ: Elsevier.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDietrich, R., Klein, W., & Noyau, C. ( Eds.). ( 1995 ). The acquisition of temporality in a second language. Amsterdam: Benjamins.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceElio, R., & Anderson, J. R. ( 1981 ). The effects of category generalizations and instance similarity on schema abstraction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 7 ( 6 ), 397 – 417.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceElio, R., & Anderson, J. R. ( 1984 ). The effects of information order and learning mode on schema abstraction. Memory & Cognition, 12, 20 – 30.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEllis, N. C. ( 1998 ). Emergentism, connectionism and language learning. Language Learning, 48, 631 – 664.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEllis, N. C. ( 2003 ). Constructions, chunking, and connectionism: The emergence of second language structure. In C. Doughty & M. H. Long ( Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEllis, N. C. ( 2006a ). Cognitive perspectives on SLA: The associative cognitive CREED. AILA Review, 19, 100 – 121.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEllis, N. C. ( 2006b ). Language acquisition as rational contingency learning. Applied Linguistics, 27, 1 – 24.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEllis, N. C. ( 2008 ). The psycholinguistics of the interaction hypothesis. In A. Mackey & C. Polio ( Eds.), Multiple perspectives on interaction in SLA: Second language research in honor of Susan M. Gass ( pp. 11 – 40 ). New York: Routledge.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEllis, N. C., & Ferreira-Junior, F. ( in press ). Slots, frames, and the distinctiveness of their occupants: Island populations in the acquisition of verb argument constructions. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEllis, N. C., & Larsen-Freeman, D. ( in press-a ). Emergent models of the acquisition of verb argument constructions. Language Learning, 59 ( Suppl. 1 ).en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEllis, N. C., & Larsen-Freeman, D. ( in press-b ). Language as a Complex Adaptive System [ Special Issue ]. Language Learning, 59 ( Suppl. 1 ).en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFeldweg, H. ( 1991 ). The European Science Foundation Second Language Database. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGass, S. ( 1997 ). Input, interaction, and the development of second languages. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGass, S. ( 2003 ). Input and interaction. In C. Doughty & M. Long ( Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition ( pp. 224 – 255 ). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGass, S., & Mackey, A. ( 2007 ). Input, interaction and output: An overview. AILA Review, 19, 3 – 17.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGass, S., & Varonis, E. ( 1994 ). Input, interaction and second language production. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 283 – 302.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGoldberg, A. E. ( 1995 ). Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGoldberg, A. E. ( 1999 ). The emergence of argument structure semantics. In B. MacWhinney ( Ed.), The emergence of language ( pp. 197 – 212 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGoldberg, A. E. ( 2003 ). Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 219 – 224.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGoldberg, A. E. ( 2006 ). Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGoldberg, A. E., Casenhiser, D. M., & Sethuraman, N. ( 2004 ). Learning argument structure generalizations. Cognitive Linguistics, 15, 289 – 316.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGranger, S. ( 2001 ). Prefabricated patterns in advanced EFL writing: Collocations and formulae. In A. P. Cowie ( Ed.), Phraseology: Theory, analysis, and applications. Oxford: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGries, S. T., & Stefanowitsch, A. ( 2004 ). Extending collostructional analysis: A corpus-based perspective on “alternations.” International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 9, 97 – 129.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceISLE Metadata Initiative. ( 2009 ). IMDI Browser 3.0. Retrieved from Max Planck Institute Web site: http://corpus1.mpi.nl/ds/imdi_browser/en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKennedy, G. ( 2003 ). Amplifier collocations in the British National Corpus: Implications for English language teaching. TESOL Quarterly, 37, 477 – 486.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKlein, W., & Purdue, C. ( 1992 ). Utterance structure: Developing grammars again. Amsterdam: Benjamins.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLakoff, G. ( 1987 ). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLangacker, R. W. ( 1987 ). Foundations of cognitive grammar: Vol. 1. Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLong, M. H. ( 1980 ). Input, interaction, and second language acquiistion. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMackey, A., & Gass, S. ( 2006 ). Pushing the methodological boundaries in interaction research: An introduction to the special issue. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 169 – 178.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMacWhinney, B. ( 2000a ). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk: Vol. 1. Transcription format and programs ( 3rd ed. ). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMacWhinney, B. ( 2000b ). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk: Vol. 2. The database ( 3rd ed. ). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMacWhinney, B. ( 2007 ). The TalkBank Project. In J. C. Beal, K. P. Corrigan, & H. L. Moisl ( Eds.), Creating and digitizing language corpora: Synchronic databases ( Vol. 1, pp. 163 – 180 ). Houndmills, UK: Palgrave-Macmillan.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceManes, J., & Wolfson, N. ( 1989 ). The compliment formula. In F. Coulmas ( Ed.), Conversational routine. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMurphy, G. L. ( 2003 ). The big book of concepts. Boston: MIT Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNinio, A. ( 1999 ). Pathbreaking verbs in syntactic development and the question of prototypical transitivity. Journal of Child Language, 26, 619 – 653.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNinio, A. ( 2006 ). Language and the learning curve: A new theory of syntactic development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceOrtega, L., & Iberri-Shea, G. ( 2005 ). Longitudinal research in second language acquisition: Recent trends and future directions. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 25, 26 – 45.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePawley, A., & Syder, F. H. ( 1983 ). Two puzzles for linguistic theory: Nativelike selection and nativelike fluency. In J. C. Richards & R. W. Schmidt ( Eds.), Language and communication ( pp. 191 – 225 ). London: Longman.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePerdue, C. ( Ed.). ( 1993 ). Adult language acquisition: Crosslinguistic perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePickering, M. J. ( 2006 ). The dance of dialogue. The Psychologist, 19, 734 – 737.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. C. ( 2006 ). Alignment as the basis for successful communication. Research on Language and Computation, 4, 203 – 228.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePinker, S. ( 1989 ). Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePosner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. ( 1968 ). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 353 – 363.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePosner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. ( 1970 ). Retention of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 304 – 308.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRobinson, P., & Ellis, N. C. ( Eds.). ( 2008 ). A handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition. London: Routledge.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. ( 1975 ). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192 – 233.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. ( 1976 ). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382 – 439.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRosch, E., Varela, F., & Thompson, E. ( 1991 ). The embodied mind. Boston: MIT Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceShanks, D. R. ( 1995 ). The psychology of associative learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSinclair, J. ( 1991 ). Corpus, concordance, collocation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStefanowitsch, A., & Gries, S. T. ( 2003 ). Collostructions: Investigating the interaction between words and constructions. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 8, 209 – 243.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTalkbank. ( 2007 ). European Science Foundation corpus. Retrieved from Talkbank Web site: http://talkbank.org/data/BilingBank/en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTheakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. ( 2004 ). Semantic generality, input frequency and the acquisition of syntax. Journal of Child Language, 31, 61 – 99.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTomasello, M. ( 1992 ). First verbs: A case study of early grammatical development. New York: Cambridge University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTomasello, M. ( 2003 ). Constructing a language. Boston: Harvard University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTomasello, M., & Stahl, D. ( 2004 ). Sampling children's spontaneous speech: How much is enough? Journal of Child Language, 31, 101 – 121.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWulff, S., Ellis, N. C., RÖmer, U., Bardovi-Harlig, K., & LeBlanc, C. ( 2009 ). The acquisition of tense–aspect: Converging evidence from corpora and telicity ratings. Modern Language Journal, 93, 354 – 369.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceYear, J.-E., & Gordon, P. ( 2009 ). Korean speakers’ acquisition of the English ditransitive construction: The role of verb prototype, input distribution, and frequency. Modern Language Journal, 93, 399 – 417.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceZipf, G. K. ( 1935 ). The psycho-biology of language: An introduction to dynamic philology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceZipf, G. K. ( 1949 ). Human behavior and the principle of least effort. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.