Show simple item record

On the spontaneous discovery of a mathematical relation during problem solving

dc.contributor.authorDixon, James A.en_US
dc.contributor.authorBangert, Ashley S.en_US
dc.date.accessioned2010-06-01T21:28:56Z
dc.date.available2010-06-01T21:28:56Z
dc.date.issued2004-05en_US
dc.identifier.citationDixon, James A.; Bangert, Ashley S. (2004). "On the spontaneous discovery of a mathematical relation during problem solving." Cognitive Science 28(3): 433-449. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/74539>en_US
dc.identifier.issn0364-0213en_US
dc.identifier.issn1551-6709en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/74539
dc.description.abstractPeople spontaneously discover new representations during problem solving. Discovery of a mathematical representation is of special interest, because it shows that the underlying structure of the problem has been extracted. In the current study, participants solved gear-system problems as part of a game. Although none of the participants initially used a mathematical representation, many discovered a parity-based, mathematical strategy during problem solving. Two accounts of the spontaneous discovery of mathematical strategies were tested. According to the automatic schema abstraction hypothesis, experience with multiple, unique problem exemplars facilitates extraction of the parity relation. According to the comparison-based abstraction hypothesis, explicitly comparing gear pathways that have different number, but the same parity, should result in extraction of parity. An event history analysis showed that accumulation of experiences with different-number, same-parity comparisons predicted discovery of parity; accumulation of unique exemplars did not. Results suggest that comparison-based abstraction processes can lead to the discovery of a mathematical relation.en_US
dc.format.extent330972 bytes
dc.format.extent3109 bytes
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.format.mimetypetext/plain
dc.publisherLawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.en_US
dc.publisherBlackwell Publishing Ltden_US
dc.rights2004 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.en_US
dc.subject.otherSchema Abstractionen_US
dc.subject.otherDiscoveryen_US
dc.subject.otherMathematical Relationsen_US
dc.subject.otherComparisonen_US
dc.subject.otherStructure Mappingen_US
dc.subject.otherProblem Solvingen_US
dc.titleOn the spontaneous discovery of a mathematical relation during problem solvingen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelNeurosciencesen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelHealth Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherDepartment of Psychology, 406 Babbidge Road, Unit 1020, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269-1020, USAen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherDepartment of Psychology, 525 East University, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1109, USAen_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/74539/1/s15516709cog2803_6.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1207/s15516709cog2803_6en_US
dc.identifier.sourceCognitive Scienceen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAllison, P. D. ( 1984 ). Event history analysis: Regression for longitudinal event data. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBernardo, A. B. I. ( 2001 ). Principle explanation and strategic schema abstraction. Memory & Cognition, 29, 627 – 633.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBruner, J. S. ( 1966 ). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChen, Z. ( 1999 ). Schema induction in children's analogical problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 703 – 715.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDixon, J. A., & Bangert, A. ( 2002 ). The prehistory of discovery: Precursors of representational change in solving gear-system problems. Developmental Psychology, 38, 918 – 933.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDixon, J. A., & Dohn, M. C. ( 2003 ). Redescription disembeds relations: Evidence from relational transfer and use in problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 31, 1082 – 1093.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFoard, C. F., & Kemler-Nelson, D. G. ( 1984 ). Holistic and analytic modes of processing: The multiple determinants of perceptual analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 94 – 111.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGentner, D. ( 1983 ). Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7, 155 – 170.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGentner, D., & Markman, A. B. ( 1997 ). Structure mapping in analogy and similarity. American Psychologist, 52, 45 – 56.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGentner, D., & Medina, J. ( 1998 ). Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition, 65, 263 – 287.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGentner, D., & Namy, L. L. ( 1999 ). Comparison in the development of categories. Cognitive Development, 14, 487 – 513.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. ( 1983 ). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1 – 39.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGoldinger, S. D. ( 1998 ). Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. Psychological Review, 105, 251 – 279.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGoldstone, R. L., & Sakamoto, Y. ( 2003 ). The transfer of abstract principles governing complex adaptive systems. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 414 – 466.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGomez, R. L. ( 2002 ). Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychological Science, 13, 431 – 436.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHintzman, D. L. ( 1986 ). “Schema abstraction” in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, 93, 411 – 428.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHolyoak, K. J., & Thagard, P. ( 1989 ). Analogical mapping by constraint satisfaction. Cognitive Science, 13, 295 – 355.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHummel, J. E., & Holyoak, K. J. ( 1997 ). Distributed representations of structure: A theory of analogical access and mapping. Psychological Review, 104, 427 – 466.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKaplan, C. A., & Simon, H. A. ( 1990 ). In search of insight. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 374 – 419.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKeane, M. T. ( 1997 ). What makes an analogy difficult? The effects of order and causal structure on analogical mapping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 946 – 967.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKemler-Nelson, D. G. ( 1993 ). Processing integral dimensions: The whole view. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 1105 – 1113.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKotovsky, L., & Gentner, D. ( 1996 ). Comparison and categorization in the development of relational similarity. Child Development, 67, 2797 – 2822.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLoewenstein, J., & Gentner, D. ( 2001 ). Spatial mapping in preschoolers: Close comparisons facilitate far mappings. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2, 189 – 219.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMarkman, A. B., & Gentner, D. ( 1993 ). Structural alignment during similarity comparisons. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 431 – 467.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMarkman, A. B., & Gentner, D. ( 2000 ). Structure mapping in the comparison process. American Journal of Psychology, 113, 501 – 538.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMedin, D. L., & Ross, B. H. ( 1989 ). The specific character of abstract thought: Categorization, problem solving, and induction. In R. J. Sternberg ( Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence ( Vol. 5, pp. 189 – 223 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMenard, S. ( 1995 ). Applied logistic regression analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMosteller, F., & Tukey, J. W. ( 1977 ). Data analysis and regression: A second course in statistics. Reading, MA: Addision-Wesley.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNamy, L. L., & Gentner, D. ( 2002 ). Making a silk purse out of two sow's ears: Young children's use of comparison in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 5 – 15.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNational Council of Teachers of Mathematics. ( 2000 ). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceReeves, L. M., & Weisberg, R. W. ( 1994 ). The role of content and abstract information in analogical transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 381 – 400.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceReyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. ( 1995 ). Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learning and Individual Differences, 7, 1 – 75.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRoss, B. H. ( 1984 ). Remindings and their effects in learning a cognitive skill. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 371 – 416.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRoss, B. H. ( 1989 ). Remindings in learning and instruction. In S. Vosniadou & A. Ortony ( Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning ( pp. 438 – 469 ). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRoss, B. H., & Kennedy, P. T. ( 1990 ). Generalizing from the use of earlier examples in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 42 – 55.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRoss, B. H., & Warren, J. L. ( 2002 ). Learning abstract relations from using categories. Memory & Cognition, 5, 657 – 665.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRoss, G., Nelson, K., Wetstone, H., & Tanouye, E. ( 1986 ). Acquisition and generalization of novel object concepts by young language learners. Journal of Child Language, 13, 67 – 83.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSchwartz, D. L., & Black, J. B. ( 1996 ). Shuttling between depictive models and abstract rules: Induction and fallback. Cognitive Science, 20, 457 – 497.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceShrager, J., & Siegler, R. S. ( 1998 ). A model of children's strategy choices and strategy discoveries. Psychological Science, 9, 405 – 410.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSinger, J. D., & Willett, J. B. ( 2003 ). Applied longitudinal data analysis. New York: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWard, T. B. ( 1983 ). Response tempo and separable-integral responding: Evidence for an integral-to-separable processing sequence in visual perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 103 – 112.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWickens, T. D. ( 1989 ). Multiway contingency tables for the social sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.