Show simple item record

Do Physicians Know When Their Diagnoses Are Correct?

dc.contributor.authorFriedman, Charles P.en_US
dc.contributor.authorGatti, Guido G.en_US
dc.contributor.authorFranz, Timothy M.en_US
dc.contributor.authorMurphy, Gwendolyn C.en_US
dc.contributor.authorWolf, Fredric M.en_US
dc.contributor.authorHeckerling, Paul S.en_US
dc.contributor.authorFine, Paul L.en_US
dc.contributor.authorMiller, Thomas M.en_US
dc.contributor.authorElstein, Arthur S.en_US
dc.date.accessioned2010-06-01T21:48:37Z
dc.date.available2010-06-01T21:48:37Z
dc.date.issued2005-04en_US
dc.identifier.citationFriedman, Charles P.; Gatti, Guido G.; Franz, Timothy M.; Murphy, Gwendolyn C.; Wolf, Fredric M.; Heckerling, Paul S.; Fine, Paul L.; Miller, Thomas M.; Elstein, Arthur S. (2005). "Do Physicians Know When Their Diagnoses Are Correct?." Journal of General Internal Medicine 20(4): 334-339. <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/74850>en_US
dc.identifier.issn0884-8734en_US
dc.identifier.issn1525-1497en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/74850
dc.identifier.urihttp://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/sites/entrez?cmd=retrieve&db=pubmed&list_uids=15857490&dopt=citationen_US
dc.description.abstractThis study explores the alignment between physicians' confidence in their diagnoses and the “correctness” of these diagnoses, as a function of clinical experience, and whether subjects were prone to over-or underconfidence. Design : Prospective, counterbalanced experimental design. Setting : Laboratory study conducted under controlled conditions at three academic medical centers. Participants : Seventy-two senior medical students, 72 senior medical residents, and 72 faculty internists. Intervention : We created highly detailed, 2-to 4-page synopses of 36 diagnostically challenging medical cases, each with a definitive correct diagnosis. Subjects generated a differential diagnosis for each of 9 assigned cases, and indicated their level of confidence in each diagnosis. Measurements And Main Results : A differential was considered “correct” if the clinically true diagnosis was listed in that subject's hypothesis list. To assess confidence, subjects rated the likelihood that they would, at the time they generated the differential, seek assistance in reaching a diagnosis. Subjects' confidence and correctness were “mildly” aligned (Κ=.314 for all subjects, .285 for faculty, .227 for residents, and .349 for students). Residents were overconfident in 41% of cases where their confidence and correctness were not aligned, whereas faculty were overconfident in 36% of such cases and students in 25%. Conclusions : Even experienced clinicians may be unaware of the correctness of their diagnoses at the time they make them. Medical decision support systems, and other interventions designed to reduce medical errors, cannot rely exclusively on clinicians' perceptions of their needs for such support.en_US
dc.format.extent113550 bytes
dc.format.extent3109 bytes
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.format.mimetypetext/plain
dc.publisherBlackwell Science Incen_US
dc.rights© 2005 by the Society of General Internal Medicine. All rights reserveden_US
dc.subject.otherDiagnostic Reasoningen_US
dc.subject.otherClinical Decision Supporten_US
dc.subject.otherMedical Errorsen_US
dc.subject.otherClinical Judgmenten_US
dc.subject.otherConfidenceen_US
dc.titleDo Physicians Know When Their Diagnoses Are Correct?en_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelInternal Medicine and Specialtiesen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelHealth Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumDepartment of Medicine, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA ;en_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherCenter for Biomedical Informatics, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ;en_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherDepartment of Psychology, St. John Fisher College, Rochester, NY, USA ;en_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherDivision of Community Health, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA ;en_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherDepartment of Medical Education and Informatics, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA ;en_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherDepartments of Medicine anden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherMedical Education, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA ;en_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherDepartment of Medicine, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA.en_US
dc.identifier.pmid15857490en_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/74850/1/j.1525-1497.2005.30145.x.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/j.1525-1497.2005.30145.xen_US
dc.identifier.sourceJournal of General Internal Medicineen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHersh WR. “ A world of knowledge at your fingertips ”: the promise, reality, and future directions of online information retrieval. Acad Med. 1999; 74: 240 – 3.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKohn LT, Corrigan JM, Donaldson MS, eds. To Err Is Human: Building a Safer Health System. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2000.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBates DW, Gawande AA. Error in medicine: what have we learned? Ann Intern Med. 2000; 132: 763 – 7.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLeape LL, Bates DW, Cullen DJ, et al. Systems analysis of adverse drug events. ADE Prevention Study Group. JAMA. 1995; 274: 35 – 43.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWyatt JC. Clinical data systems, part 3: development and evaluation. Lancet. 1994; 344: 1682 – 7.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNorman DA. Melding mind and machine. Technol Rev. 1997; 100: 29 – 31.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChueh H, Barnett GO. “Just in time” clinical information. Acad Med. 1997; 72: 512 – 7.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHunt DL, Haynes RB, Hanna SE, Smith K. Effects of computer-based clinical decision support systems on physician performance and patient outcomes. JAMA. 1998; 280: 1339 – 46.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMiller RA. Medical diagnostic decision support systems—past, present, and future. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 1994; 1: 8 – 27.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEvans RS, Pestotnik SL, Classen DC, et al. A computer-assisted management program for antibiotics and other antiinfective agents. N Eng J Med. 1998; 338: 232 – 8.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMcDonald CJ, Overhage JM, Tierney WM, et al. The Regenstrief Medical Record System: a quarter century experience. Int J Med Inform. 1999; 54: 225 – 53.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWagner MM, Pankaskie M, Hogan W, et al. Clinical event monitoring at the University of Pittsburgh. Proc AMIA Annu Fall Symp. 1997; 188 – 92.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCimino JJ, Elhanan G, Zeng Q. Supporting infobuttons with terminological knowledge. Proc AMIA Annu Fall Symp. 1997; 528 – 32.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMiller PL. Building an expert critiquing system: ESSENTIAL-ATTENDING. Methods Inf Med. 1986; 25: 71 – 8.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTversky A, Kahneman D. Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science. 1974; 185: 1124 – 31.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLichtenstein S, Fischhoff B. Do those who know more also know more about how much they know? Organ Behav Hum Perform. 1977; 20: 159 – 83.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChristensen-Szalanski JJ, Bushyhead JB. Physicians' use of probabilistic information in a real clinical setting. J Exp Psychol. 1981; 7: 928 – 35.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTierney WM, Fitzgerald J, McHenry R, et al. Physicians' estimates of the probability of myocardial infarction in emergency room patients with chest pain. Med Decis Making. 1986; 6: 12 – 7.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFriedman CP, Elstein AS, Wolf FM, et al. Enhancement of clinicians' diagnostic reasoning by computer-based consultation: a multisite study of 2 systems. JAMA. 1999; 282: 1851 – 6.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMann D. The relationship between diagnostic accuracy and confidence in medical students. Presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Atlanta, 1993.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFriedman C, Gatti G, Elstein A, Franz T, Murphy G, Wolf F. Are Clinicians Correct When They Believe They Are Correct? Implications for Medical Decision Support. Proceedings of the Tenth World Congress on Medical Informatics. London; 2000. Medinfo. 2001; 10 ( PP. 1 ): 454 – 8.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSwets JA, Pickett RM. Evaluation of Diagnostic Systems: Methods from Signal Detection Theory. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1982.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMcCullagh P, Nelder JA. Generalized Linear Models. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Chapman and Hall; 1991.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLiang K, Zeger SL. Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika. 1986; 73: 13 – 22.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNeter J, Kutner MH, Nachstsheim CJ, Wasserman W. Applied Linear Regression Models. Chicago, IL: Irwin; 1996.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreference26.  SAS Institute Inc. SAS/STAT User's Guide, Version 8. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc.; 1999.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLandis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977; 33: 159 – 74.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.