Show simple item record

Educational Effects of a Vocabulary Intervention on Preschoolers' Word Knowledge and Conceptual Development: A Cluster‐Randomized Trial

dc.contributor.authorNeuman, Susan B.en_US
dc.contributor.authorNewman, Ellen H.en_US
dc.contributor.authorDwyer, Julie C.en_US
dc.date.accessioned2011-12-05T18:33:59Z
dc.date.available2012-08-06T15:23:30Zen_US
dc.date.issued2011-07-08en_US
dc.identifier.citationNeuman, Susan B.; Newman, Ellen H.; Dwyer, Julie (2011). "Educational Effects of a Vocabulary Intervention on Preschoolers' Word Knowledge and Conceptual Development: A Cluster‐Randomized Trial." Reading Research Quarterly 46(3). <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/88072>en_US
dc.identifier.issn0034-0553en_US
dc.identifier.issn1936-2722en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/88072
dc.description.abstractThe purpose of this study was to examine the hypothesis that helping preschoolers learn words through categorization may enhance their ability to retain words and their conceptual properties, acting as a bootstrap for self‐learning. We examined this hypothesis by investigating the effects of the World of Words instructional program, a supplemental intervention for children in preschool designed to teach word knowledge and conceptual development through taxonomic categorization and embedded multimedia. Participants in the study included 3‐ and 4‐year‐old children from 28 Head Start classrooms in 12 schools, randomly assigned to treatment and control groups. Children were assessed on word knowledge, expressive language, conceptual knowledge, and categories and properties of concepts in a yearlong intervention. Results indicated that children receiving the WOW treatment consistently outperformed their control counterparts; further, treatment children were able to use categories to identify the meaning of novel words. Gains in word and categorical knowledge were sustained six months later for those children who remained in Head Start. These results suggest that a program targeted to learning words within taxonomic categories may act as a bootstrap for self‐learning and inference generation. كان الغرض من هذه الدراسة هو فحص فرضية أن مساعدة الأطفال في الروضة تعلم كلمات عن طريق التصنيف بإمكانه أن يعزز من قدراتهم على تذكر الكلمات وخصائصها المفاهيمية، بوصفها محفزا للتعلم. قمنا بفحص هذه الفرضية وذلك بالتحقيق في أثر البرنامج التعليمي "عالم المفردات" (WOW) الذي يعتبر تدخلا تكميليا لأطفال الروضة ومصمما من أجل تعليم معرفة الكلمات والتطور المفاهيمي، من خلال النظام التصنيفي ووسائل الإعلام. تضمنت الدراسة مشاركين تتراوح أعمارهم بين 3 و4 سنوات ينتمون إلى 28 روضة أطفال، التي تنتمي إلى 12 مدرسة, خضعت عشوائيا للبحث ومراقبة المجموعات. جرى تقييم الأطفال على معرفة الكلمات واللغة التعبيرية والمعرفة المفاهيمية وأصناف وخصائص المفاهيم في تدخل لمدة سنة كاملة. أظهرت النتائج أن الأطفال الذين خضعوا للبرنامج التعليمي "عالم المفردات" (WOW) قد تفوقوا بصورة مستمرة على نظرائهم الذين خضعوا للمراقبة. علاوة على ذلك, فإن أطفال البحث قد تمكنوا من استخدام الأصناف والتعرف على معنى الكلمات الجديدة. استمر اكتساب الكلمات والمعرفة التصنيفية بعد 6 أشهر بالنسبة للأطفال الذين بقوا في روضة الأطفال. توحي هذه النتائج إلى أنه يمكن لبرنامج يهدف إلى تعلم الكلمات أن يعمل كمحفز للتعلم الذاتي، وتوليد الاستنتاجات. 本研究旨在考查一个假设:帮助学龄前儿童通过分类来学习单词,可提高他们记忆单词及其概念属性的能力,从而发展他们的自主学习能力。作者通过调查「单词世界」(WOW)教学计划的影响来考查这个假设。该教学计划是一个学龄前儿童补充干预计划,旨在透过使用分类学的分类方法及嵌入式多媒体,教授单词知识和发展单词概念。研究参与者是来自12所学校中的28个「启蒙计划」学前儿童班里的3‐4岁儿童,他们被随机分配到干预组和对照组。在一年的干预中,儿童接受单词知识、表达语言、概念知识、类别和概念属性的评估。结果显示,「单词世界」(WOW)干预组的表现一致地优于对照组;此外,干预组儿童可以使用类别来确定新词的词义。仍然留在「启蒙计划」班里的儿童,其单词和类别知识的改进保持稳定至干预后6个月。这些研究结果显示,针对透过分类学的分类方法来学习单词的教学计划,可引导儿童凭自己的力量学习自学和产生推断。 Cette étude avait pour but d'examiner l'hypothíse qu'aider des enfants d'âge préscolaire à apprendre des mots en les catégorisant pourrait favoriser leur capacité à retenir les mots et leurs propriétés, agissant alors comme une amorce d'auto‐apprentissage. Nous avons examiné cette hypothíse en analysant les effets du matériel d'enseignement le Monde des Mots (MDM), un supplément pédagogique destiné aux enfants d'âge préscolaire conçu pour la connaissance des mots et le développement des concepts à l'aide d'une catégorisation taxinomique avec multimédia impliqué. Ont participé à l'étude des enfants de 3 et 4 ans provenant de 28 classes Head Start issus de 12 écoles assignées au hasard au groupe de traitement et au groupe contrôle. Les enfants ont été évalués sur leur connaissance des mots, l'expression orale, les connaissances conceptuelles, les catégories et les propriétés des concepts tout au long de l'année de l'intervention. Les résultats ont montré que les enfants du groupe de traitement MDM ont de maniíre systématique dépassé leur contrepartie du groupe contrôle; en outre, les enfants soumis au traitement ont été en mesure de se servir des catégories pour trouver le sens des mots nouveaux. Les bénéfices dans la connaissance des mots et les connaissances catégorielles sont demeurés six mois plus tard chez les enfants restés dans Head Start. Ces résultats suggírent qu'un programme visant l'apprentissage de mots au sein de catégories taxonomiques peut agir comme une amorce pour l'auto‐apprentissage et la production d'inférences. Проверялась гипотеза о том, что категоризация лексики при обучении дошкольников новым словам может существенно увеличить способность к запоминанию слов и их концептуальных свойств и стимулировать малышей к самообучению. Для расширения их словарного запаса и умения работать с концептами авторы исследовали учебный модуль “Мир слов” (WOW), разработанный в качестве дополнительного вмешательства для дошкольников, которые обучаются по программе Head Start. WOW знакомит детей с таксономической классификацией посредством мультимедийных средств. Трех‐ и четырехлетние дети из 28‐и дошкольных групп в 12‐и школах были случайным образом включены либо в экспериментальную, либо в контрольную группу. На протяжении годичного обучения оценивалось знание слов, выразительность речи, знание концептов, их свойств и категорий, к которым они могут быть причислены. Дети, обучавшиеся по программе WOW, стабильно показывали более высокие результаты, чем их ровесники из контрольных групп. Помимо прочего, эти дети способны использовать категоризацию для определения значений новых слов. Через полгода после окончания обучения эти дети продолжали опережать сверстников по знанию слов и умению категоризировать. Это свидетельствует о том, что программа, предлагающая изучение слов в рамках таксономических категорий, может помочь вырастить поколение, которое будет способно к самообучению и к самостоятельным выводам. La meta de este estudio fue el de investigar la hipótesis que ayudar a los preescolares a aprender palabras por medio de la categorización mejoraría su capacidad de retener palabras y sus propiedades conceptuales, sirviendo de arranque para el auto aprendizaje. Investigamos esta hipótesis estudiando los efectos del programa de enseñanza World of Words (Mundo de palabras; WOW por sus siglas en inglés), una intervención adicional para niños preescolares diseñada para el aprendizaje de palabras y el desarrollo conceptual por medio de la categorización taxonómica y el uso de diversos medios. En este estudio participaron niños de 3 y 4 años de 20 aulas de Head Start en 12 escuelas escogidas al azar en cuanto a grupos de tratamiento y de control. En un año completo de intervención, se evaluaron los estudiantes en cuanto a su conocimiento de palabras, su lenguaje expresivo, su conocimiento conceptual, y las categorías y propiedades de los conceptos. Los resultados mostraron que los niños del grupo de WOW sistemáticamente superaban a los niños del grupo de control; además, los niños del grupo de tratamiento podían usar categorías para encontrar el significado de palabras nuevas. Los adelantos en el conocimiento de palabras y categorías todavía existían 6 meses más tarde para los niños que seguían con Head Start. Estos resultados sugieren que un programa dedicado al aprendizaje de palabras dentro de categorías taxonómicas puede ayudar al autoaprendizaje y la producción de inferencias.en_US
dc.publisherBlackwell Publishing Ltden_US
dc.publisherWiley Periodicals, Inc.en_US
dc.subject.otherTopicen_US
dc.subject.otherAssessmenten_US
dc.subject.otherCriterionen_US
dc.subject.otherOutcomesen_US
dc.subject.otherStandardizeden_US
dc.subject.otherOral Languageen_US
dc.subject.otherExpressiveen_US
dc.subject.otherAcquisitionen_US
dc.subject.otherDevelopmenten_US
dc.subject.otherReceptiveen_US
dc.subject.otherStrategiesen_US
dc.subject.otherInstructionalen_US
dc.subject.otherTechnologyen_US
dc.subject.otherProgramen_US
dc.subject.otherStrugglingen_US
dc.subject.otherGapen_US
dc.subject.otherFactorsen_US
dc.subject.otherInterventionen_US
dc.subject.otherTheoreticalen_US
dc.subject.otherCognitiveen_US
dc.subject.otherVocabularyen_US
dc.subject.otherGeneralen_US
dc.subject.otherSpecializeden_US
dc.subject.otherLearneren_US
dc.subject.otherEarly Childhooden_US
dc.subject.otherTypeen_US
dc.subject.otherArticleen_US
dc.titleEducational Effects of a Vocabulary Intervention on Preschoolers' Word Knowledge and Conceptual Development: A Cluster‐Randomized Trialen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollowen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelEducationen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumUniversity of Michigan Ann Arbor USAen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherIE University Segovia Spainen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherBoston University Massachusetts USAen_US
dc.identifier.pmid21658105en_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/88072/1/RRQ.46.3.3.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1598/RRQ.46.3.3en_US
dc.identifier.sourceReading Research Quarterlyen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAnderson, R.C., & Freebody, P. ( 1981 ). Vocabulary knowledge. In J.T. Guthrie (Ed.), Comprehension and teaching: Research reviews (pp. 77 – 117 ). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBeals, D.E., DeTemple, J.M., & Dickinson, D.K. ( 1994 ). Talking and listening that support early literacy development of children from low‐income families. In D.K. Dickinson (Ed.), Bridges to literacy: Children, families, and schools (pp. 19 – 40 ). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBeck, I.L., & McKeown, M.G. ( 2007 ). Increasing young low‐income children's oral vocabulary repertoires through rich and focused instruction. The Elementary School Journal, 107 ( 3 ), 251 – 271.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBeck, I.L., McKeown, M.G., & Kucan, L. ( 2002 ). Bringing words to life: Robust vocabulary instruction. New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBiemiller, A. ( 2006 ). Vocabulary development and instruction: A prerequisite for school learning. In D.K. Dickinson & S.B. Neuman (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 2, pp. 41 – 51 ). New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBiemiller, A., & Boote, C. ( 2006 ). An effective method for building meaning vocabulary in primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98 ( 1 ), 44 – 62. doi:10.1037/0022‐0663.98.1.44.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBooth, A.E. ( 2009 ). Causal supports for early word learning. Child Development, 80 ( 4 ), 1243 – 1250. doi:10.1111/j.1467‐8624.2009.01328.x.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBorovsky, A., & Elman, J. ( 2006 ). Language input and semantic categories: A relation between cognition and early word learning. Journal of Child Language, 33 ( 4 ), 759 – 790. doi:10.1017/S0305000906007574.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBos, C., & Anders, P. ( 1990 ). Effects of interactive vocabulary instruction on the vocabulary learning and reading comprehension of junior‐high learning disabled students. Learning Disability Quarterly, 13 ( 1 ), 31 – 42.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBrown, A.L., Ash, D., Rutherford, M., Nakagawa, K., Gordon, A., & Campione, J.C. ( 1993 ). Distributed expertise in the classroom. In G. Salomon (Ed.), Distributed cognitions: Psychological and educational considerations (pp. 188 – 228 ). New York: Cambridge University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBus, A.G., van Ijzendoorn, M.H., & Pellegrini, A.D. ( 1995 ). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A meta‐analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65 ( 1 ), 1 – 21.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCain, K., Oakhill, J.V., Barnes, M.A., & Bryant, P.E. ( 2001 ). Comprehension skill, inference‐making ability, and their relation to knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 29 ( 6 ), 850 – 859. doi:10.3758/BF03196414.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarey, S. ( 1988 ). Conceptual differences between children and adults. Mind & Language, 3 ( 3 ), 167 – 181. doi:10.1111/j.1468‐0017.1988.tb00141.x.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChambers, B., Abrami, P., Tucker, B., Slavin, R.E., Madden, N.A., Cheung, A., et al. ( 2008 ). Computer‐assisted tutoring in Success for All: Reading outcomes for first graders. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 1 ( 2 ), 120 – 137.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChambers, B., Cheung, A.C.K., Madden, N.A., Slavin, R.E., & Gifford, R. ( 2006 ). Achievement effects of embedded multimedia in a Success for All reading program. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98 ( 1 ), 232 – 237. doi:10.1037/0022‐0663.98.1.232.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChi, M.T., & Koeske, R.D. ( 1983 ). Network representation of a child's dinosaur knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 19 ( 1 ), 29 – 39. doi:10.1037/0012‐1649.19.1.29.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCoyne, M.D., McCoach, D.B., & Knapp, S. ( 2007 ). Vocabulary intervention for kindergarten students: Comparing extended instruction to embedded instruction and incidental exposure. Learning Disability Quarterly, 30 ( 2 ), 74 – 88.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCoyne, M.D., McCoach, D.B., Loftus, S., Zipoli, R., Jr., Ruby, M., Crevecoeur, Y.C., et al. ( 2010 ). Direct and extended vocabulary instruction in kindergarten: Investigating transfer effects. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 3 ( 2 ), 93 – 120.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCunningham, A.E., & Stanovich, K.E. ( 1997 ). Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later. Developmental Psychology, 33 ( 6 ), 934 – 945. doi:10.1037/0012‐1649.33.6.934.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDale, E., & O'Rourke, J. ( 1981 ). The living word vocabulary: A national vocabulary inventory. Chicago: World Book/Childcraft International.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDale, P.S., & Fenson, L. ( 1996 ). Lexical development norms for young children. Behavior Research Methods, 28 ( 1 ), 125 – 127. doi:10.3758/BF03203646.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDeBruin‐Parecki, A., & Hohmann, M. ( 2006 ). Growing readers early literacy curriculum ( 2nd ed. ) [Kit]. Ypsilanti, MI: HighScope Educational Research Foundation.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencede Villiers, J., & Johnson, V. ( 2007 ). Implications of new vocabulary assessments for minority children. In R.K. Wagner, A.E. Muse, & K.R. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 157 – 181 ). New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDickinson, D.K., McCabe, A., & Essex, M.J. ( 2006 ). A window of opportunity we must open to all: The case for preschool with high‐quality support for language and literacy. In D.K. Dickinson & S.B. Neuman (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 2, pp. 11 – 28 ). New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDyson, A.H. ( 1993 ). Social worlds of children learning to write in an urban primary school. New York: Teachers College Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceElleman, A.M., Lindo, E.J., Morphy, P., & Compton, D.L. ( 2009 ). The impact of vocabulary instruction on passage‐level comprehension of school‐age children: A meta‐analysis. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 2 ( 1 ), 1 – 44. doi:10.1080/19345740802539200.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFarkas, G., & Beron, K. ( 2004 ). The detailed age trajectory of oral vocabulary knowledge: Differences by class and race. Social Science Research, 33 ( 3 ), 464 – 497. doi:10.1016/j.ssresearch.2003.08.001.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFinn, C.E., Jr., Julian, L., & Petrilli, M.J. ( 2006 ). The state of state standards 2006. Washington, DC: Thomas B. Fordham Foundation.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S.A. ( 2003 ). The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. New York: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195154061.001.0001.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S.A., Coley, J.D., Rosengren, K.S., Hartman, E., Pappas, A., & Keil, F.C. ( 1998 ). Beyond labeling: The role of maternal input in the acquisition of richly structured categories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63 ( 1, Serial No. 253).en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S.A., & Kalish, C.W. ( 2006 ). Conceptual development. In W. Damon, R.M. Lerner (Series Eds.), D. Kuhn, & R. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 2: Cognition, perception, and language ( 6th ed., pp. 687 – 733 ). New York: John Wiley & Sons.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S.A., & O'Reilly, A.W. ( 1988 ). Children's inductive inferences within superordinate categories: The role of language and category structure. Child Development, 59 ( 4 ), 876 – 887.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGershkoff‐Stowe, L., & Hahn, E.R. ( 2007 ). Fast mapping skills in the developing lexicon. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50 ( 3 ), 682 – 697. doi:10.1044/1092‐4388(2007/048).en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGlaser, R. ( 1984 ). Education and thinking: The role of knowledge. American Psychologist, 39 ( 2 ), 93 – 104. doi:10.1037/0003‐066X.39.2.93.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. ( 1987 ). The development of categorization in the second year and its relation to other cognitive and linguistic developments. Child Development, 58 ( 6 ), 1523 – 1531.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHarris, J., Golinkoff, R.M., & Hirsh‐Pasek, K. ( 2011 ). Lessons from the crib for the classroom: How children really learn vocabulary. In S.B. Neuman & D.K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 3, pp. 49 – 65 ). New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHart, B., & Risley, T.R. ( 1995 ). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHart, B., & Risley, T.R. ( 2003 ). The early catastrophe: The 30 million word gap. American Educator, 27 ( 1 ), 4 – 9.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHedges, L.V., & Olkin, I. ( 1985 ). Statistical methods for meta‐analysis. San Diego, CA: Academic.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHoff, E. ( 2003 ). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74 ( 5 ), 1368 – 1378. doi:10.1111/1467‐8624.00612.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHohmann, M., & Weikart, D.P. ( 1995 ). Educating young children: Active learning practices for preschool and child care programs. Ypsilanti, MI: HighScope Educational Research Foundation.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJuel, C., Biancarosa, G., Coker, D., & Deffes, R. ( 2003 ). Walking with Rosie: A cautionary tale of early reading instruction. Educational Leadership, 60 ( 7 ), 12 – 18.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKaefer, T. ( 2009, December ). Conceptual support for vocabulary in preschool: Taxonomic category information scaffolds word learning for vulnerable children. Paper presented at the 59th annual meeting of the National Reading Conference, Albuquerque, NM.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKozma, R.B. ( 1991 ). Learning with media. Review of Educational Research, 61 ( 2 ), 179 – 211.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLevin, J.R. ( 1988 ). Elaboration‐based learning strategies: Powerful theory = powerful application. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 13 ( 3 ), 191 – 205. doi:10.1016/0361‐476X(88)90020‐3.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMacWhinney, B. ( 2000 ). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk. Vol. 1: Transcription format and programs ( 3rd ed. ). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMarulis, L.M., & Neuman, S.B. ( 2010 ). The effects of vocabulary intervention on young children's word learning: A meta‐analysis. Review of Educational Research, 80 ( 3 ), 300 – 335. doi:10.3102/0034654310377087.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMayer, R.E. ( 2001 ). Multimedia learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMayer, R.E., & Moreno, R. ( 2002 ). Animation as an aid to multimedia learning. Educational Psychology Review, 14 ( 1 ), 87 – 99. doi:10.1023/A:1013184611077.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMcMurray, B. ( 2007 ). Defusing the childhood vocabulary explosion. Science, 317 ( 5838 ), 631.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMedin, D.L., Lynch, E.B., & Solomon, K.O. ( 2000 ). Are there kinds of concepts? Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 121 – 147. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.121.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMetsala, J.L., & Walley, A.C. ( 1998 ). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J.L. Metsala & L.C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89 – 120 ). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMiller, P.J., Cho, G.E., & Bracey, J.R. ( 2005 ). Working‐class children's experience through the prism of personal storytelling. Human Development, 48 ( 3 ), 115 – 135. doi:10.1159/000085515.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMol, S.E., Bus, A.G., & de Jong, M.T. ( 2009 ). Interactive book reading in early education: A tool to stimulate print knowledge as well as oral language. Review of Educational Research, 79 ( 2 ), 979 – 1007. doi:10.3102/0034654309332561.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMol, S.E., Bus, A.G., de Jong, M.T., & Smeets, D.J.H. ( 2008 ). Added value of dialogic parent‐child book readings: A meta‐analysis. Early Education and Development, 19 ( 1 ), 7 – 26.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNational Early Literacy Panel. ( 2008 ). Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNational Institute for Child Health and Human Development. ( 2000 ). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence‐based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction (NIH Publication No. 00‐4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNelson, D.G.K., O'Neil, K.A., & Asher, Y.M. ( 2008 ). A mutually facilitative relationship between learning names and learning concepts in preschool children: The case of artifacts. Journal of Cognition and Development, 9 ( 2 ), 171 – 193. doi:10.1080/15248370802022621.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNeuman, S.B. ( 1995 ). Literacy in the television age: The myth of the TV effect ( 2nd ed. ). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNeuman, S.B. ( 2009 ). Changing the odds for children at risk: Seven essential principles of educational programs that break the cycle of poverty. New York: Teachers College Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNeuman, S.B., & Dwyer, J. ( 2009 ). Missing in action: Vocabulary instruction in pre‐K. The Reading Teacher, 62 ( 5 ), 384 – 392. doi:10.1598/RT.62.5.2.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNeuman, S.B., & Dwyer, J. (in press). Developing vocabulary and conceptual knowledge for low‐income preschoolers: A design experiment. Journal of Literacy Research.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNeuman, S.B., Dwyer, J., Koh, S., & Wright, T. ( 2007 ). The World of Words (WOW): A vocabulary intervention for low‐income preschoolers [Curriculum ]. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNeuman, S.B., & Roskos, K. ( 1992 ). Literacy objects as cultural tools: Effects on children's literacy behaviors in play. Reading Research Quarterly, 27 ( 3 ), 202 – 225.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNeuman, S.B., & Roskos, K. (with Wright, T., & Lenhart, L. ). ( 2007 ). Nurturing knowledge: Building a foundation for school success by linking early literacy to math, science, art, and social studies. New York: Scholastic.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePaivio, A. ( 1986 ). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRehder, B., & Hastie, R. ( 2004 ). Category coherence and category‐based property induction. Cognition, 91 ( 2 ), 113 – 153. doi:10.1016/S0010‐0277(03)00167‐7.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRosch, E., Mervis, C.B., Gray, W.D., Johnson, D.M., & Boyes‐Braem, P. ( 1976 ). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 438 – 439.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceScarborough, H.S. ( 2002 ). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In S.B. Neuman & D.K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 97 – 110 ). New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceScarborough, H.S., & Dobrich, W. ( 1994 ). On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Developmental Review, 14 ( 3 ), 245 – 302. doi:10.1006/drev.1994.1010.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceScott, J.A., Jamieson‐Noel, D., & Asselin, M. ( 2003 ). Vocabulary instruction throughout the day in twenty‐three Canadian upper‐elementary classrooms. The Elementary School Journal, 103 ( 3 ), 269 – 286.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSénéchal, M., Ouellette, G., & Rodney, D. ( 2006 ). The misunderstood giant: On the predictive role of early vocabulary to future reading. In D.K. Dickinson & S.B. Neuman (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 2, pp. 173 – 182 ). New York: Guilford.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSilverman, R., & Hines, S. ( 2009 ). The effects of multimedia‐enhanced instruction on the vocabulary of English‐language learners and non‐English‐language learners in pre‐kindergarten through second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101 ( 2 ), 305 – 314. doi:10.1037/a0014217.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSmiley, S.S., & Brown, A.L. ( 1979 ). Conceptual preference for thematic or taxonomic relations: A nonmonotonic age trend from preschool to old age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 28 ( 2 ), 249 – 257. doi:10.1016/0022‐0965(79)90087‐0.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSpycher, P. ( 2009 ). Learning academic language through science in two linguistically diverse kindergarten classes. The Elementary School Journal, 109 ( 4 ), 359 – 379.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStahl, S.A., & Fairbanks, M.M. ( 1986 ). The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model‐based meta‐analysis. Review of Educational Research, 56 ( 1 ), 72 – 110.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStahl, S.A., & Nagy, W.E. ( 2006 ). Teaching word meanings. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStanovich, K.E. ( 1986 ). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21 ( 4 ), 360 – 407. doi:10.1598/RRQ.21.4.1.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStockman, I.J. ( 2010 ). A review of developmental and applied language research on African American children: From a deficit to difference perspective on dialect differences. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41 ( 1 ), 23 – 38. doi:10.1044/0161‐1461(2009/08‐0086).en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceStorch, S.A., & Whitehurst, G.J. ( 2002 ). Oral language and code‐related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38 ( 6 ), 934 – 947. doi:10.1037/0012‐1649.38.6.934.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTizard, B., & Hughes, M. ( 1984 ). Young children learning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWalberg, H.J., & Tsai, S. ( 1983 ). Matthew effects in education. American Educational Research Journal, 20 ( 3 ), 359 – 373.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWellman, H.M., & Gelman, S.A. ( 1998 ). Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), D. Kuhn, & R.S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 2: Cognition, perception, and language ( 5th ed., pp. 523 – 573 ). New York: John Wiley & Sons.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWilkinson, K.M., Ross, E., & Diamond, A. ( 2003 ). Fast mapping of multiple words: Insights into when “the information provided” does and does not equal “the information perceived.” Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24 ( 6 ), 739 – 762. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2003.09.006.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWright, T., & Neuman, S.B. ( 2010, April ). Explicit vocabulary instruction in kindergarten. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Michigan Reading Association, Grand Rapids.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKeats, E.J. ( 1999 ). Over in the meadow. New York: Puffin.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.