Show simple item record

I’ll have what she’s having: the impact of model characteristics on children’s food choices

dc.contributor.authorFrazier, Brandy N.en_US
dc.contributor.authorGelman, Susan A.en_US
dc.contributor.authorKaciroti, Niko A.en_US
dc.contributor.authorRussell, Joshua W.en_US
dc.contributor.authorLumeng, Julie C.en_US
dc.date.accessioned2012-03-16T16:01:51Z
dc.date.available2013-03-04T15:29:56Zen_US
dc.date.issued2012-01en_US
dc.identifier.citationFrazier, Brandy N.; Gelman, Susan A.; Kaciroti, Niko; Russell, Joshua W.; Lumeng, Julie C. (2012). "I’ll have what she’s having: the impact of model characteristics on children’s food choices." Developmental Science 15(1). <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/90408>en_US
dc.identifier.issn1363-755Xen_US
dc.identifier.issn1467-7687en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/90408
dc.description.abstractThis research investigates children’s use of social categories in their food selection. Across three studies, we presented preschoolers with sets of photographs that contrasted food‐eating models with different characteristics, including model gender, race (Black, White), age (child or adult), and/or expression (acceptance or rejection of the food). Children were asked to pick between the photographs to choose which food they would like for snack. Results demonstrated that preschoolers prefer foods being eaten by models with positive over negative expressions, foods being eaten by child over adult models, and foods being eaten by child models of the same gender as themselves over models of the other gender. This work connects with previous research on children’s understanding of social categories and also has important practical implications for how characteristics of a food‐eating model can affect children’s willingness to try new foods.en_US
dc.publisherWiley Periodicals, Inc.en_US
dc.publisherBlackwell Publishing Ltden_US
dc.titleI’ll have what she’s having: the impact of model characteristics on children’s food choicesen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollowen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPediatricsen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelHealth Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationum. Department of Psychology,University of Michigan, USAen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationother. Department of Psychology, University of Hawaii at Manoa, USAen_US
dc.identifier.pmid22251295en_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/90408/1/j.1467-7687.2011.01106.x.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01106.xen_US
dc.identifier.sourceDevelopmental Scienceen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceQuintana, S.M. ( 1998 ). Children’s developmental understanding of ethnicity and race. Applied & Preventative Psychology, 7, 27 – 45.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLiang, K.‐Y., & Zeger, S.L. ( 1986 ). Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika, 73, 13 – 22.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLumeng, J.C., Cardinal, T.M., Jankowski, M., Kaciroti, N., & Gelman, S.A. ( 2008 ). Children’s use of adult testimony to guide food selection. Appetite, 51, 302 – 310.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLutz, D.R., & Keil, F.C. ( 2002 ). Early understanding of the division of cognitive labor. Child Development, 73, 1073 – 1084.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMcIntosh, W.A., & Zey, M. ( 1989 ). Women as gatekeepers of food consumption: a sociological critique. Food and Foodways, 34, 317 – 332.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMartin, C.L., & Halverson, C.F. ( 1981 ). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52, 1119 – 1134.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMoses, L.J., Baldwin, D.A., Rosicky, J.G., & Tidball, G. ( 2001 ). Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twelve and eighteen months. Child Development, 72, 718 – 735.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMumme, D., & Fernald, A. ( 2003 ). The infant as onlooker: learning from emotional reactions observed in a televised scenario. Child Development, 74, 221 – 237.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNguyen, S.P., & McCullough, M.B. ( 2009 ). Evaluative categorization of food in children and adults. In S.J. Ellsworth & R.C. Schuster (Eds.), Appetite and nutritional assessment (pp. 175 – 187 ). New York: Nova Science Publishers.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRhodes, M., & Gelman, S.A. ( 2009 ). A developmental examination of the conceptual structure of animal, artifact, and human social categories across two cultural contexts. Cognitive Psychology, 59, 244 – 274.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRousset, S., Schlich, P., Chatonnier, A., Barthomeuf, L., & Droit‐Volet, S. ( 2007 ). Is the desire to eat familiar and unfamiliar meat products influenced by the emotions expressed on eaters’ faces? Appetite, 50, 110 – 119.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C.R. ( 2008 ). Disgust. In M. Lewis, J.M. Haviland‐Jones, & L.F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions ( 3rd edn., pp. 757 – 776 ). New York: Guilford Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRuble, D.N., Balaban, T., & Cooper, J. ( 1981 ). Gender constancy and the effects of sex‐typed televised toy commercials. Child Development, 52, 667 – 673.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceShutts, K., Banaji, M.R., & Spelke, E.S. ( 2010 ). Social categories guide young children’s preferences for novel objects. Developmental Science, 13, 599 – 610.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceShutts, K., Kinzler, K.D., McKee, C.B., & Spelke, E.S. ( 2009 ). Social information guides infants’ selection of foods. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10, 1 – 17.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTaylor, M.G. ( 1996 ). The development of children’s beliefs about social and biological aspects of gender differences. Child Development, 67, 1555 – 1571.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTaylor, M., Cartwright, B.S., & Bowden, T. ( 1991 ). Perspective taking and theory of mind: do children predict interpretive diversity as a function of differences in observers’ knowledge? Child Development, 62, 1334 – 1351.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceVaish, A., Grossmann, T., & Woodward, A. ( 2008 ). Not all emotions are created equal: the negativity bias in social‐emotional development. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 383 – 403.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceVanderBorght, M., & Jaswal, V.K. ( 2009 ). Who knows best? Preschoolers sometimes prefer child informants over adult informants. Infant and Child Development, 18, 61 – 71.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceZeger, S.L., & Liang, K.‐Y. ( 1986 ). Longitudinal data analysis for discrete and continuous outcomes. Biometrics, 42, 121 – 130.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceZeinstra, G.G., Koelen, M.A., Colindres, D., Kok, F.J., & de Graaf, C. ( 2009 ). Facial expressions in school‐aged children are a good indicator of ‘dislikes’, but not of ‘likes’. Food Quality and Preference, 20, 620 – 624.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAboud, F.E. ( 2003 ). The formation of in‐group favoritism and out‐group prejudice in young children: are they distinct attitudes? Developmental Psychology, 39, 48 – 60.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAddessi, E., Galloway, A.T., Visalberghi, E., & Birch, L.L. ( 2005 ). Specific social influences on the acceptance of novel foods in 2–5‐year‐old children. Appetite, 45, 264 – 271.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAldridge, V., Dovey, T.M., & Halford, J.C. ( 2009 ). The role of familiarity in dietary development. Developmental Review, 29, 32 – 44.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBandura, A. ( 1977 ). Social learning theory. New York: General Learning Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBirch, L.L. ( 1980 ). Effects of peer models’ food choices and eating behaviors on preschoolers’ food preferences. Child Development, 51, 489 – 496.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBirch, L.L., & Fisher, J.O. ( 1998 ). Development of eating behaviors among children and adolescents. Pediatrics, 101, 539 – 549.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBrody, G.H., & Stoneman, Z. ( 1981 ). Selective imitation of same‐age, older, and younger peer models. Child Development, 52, 717 – 720.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCashdan, E. ( 1994 ). A sensitive period for learning about food. Human Nature, 5, 279 – 291.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCashdan, E. ( 1998 ). Adaptiveness of food learning and food aversions in children. Social Science Information, 37, 613 – 632.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Kurzban, R. ( 2003 ). Perceptions of race. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 173 – 179.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDovey, T.M., Staples, P.A., Gibson, E.L., & Halford, J.C. ( 2008 ). Food neophobia and ‘picky/fussy’ eating in children: a review. Appetite, 50, 181 – 193.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDuncker, K. ( 1938 ). Experimental modification of children’s food preferences through social suggestion. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 33, 489 – 507.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFawcett, C., & Markson, L. ( 2010a ). Children reason about shared preferences. Developmental Psychology, 46, 299 – 309.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFawcett, C., & Markson, L. ( 2010b ). Similarity predicts liking in 3‐year‐old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 345 – 358.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFeese, M., Franklin, F., Murdock, M., Harrington, K., Brown‐Binns, M., Nicklas, T., Hughes, S., & Morales, M. ( 2003 ). Prevalence of obesity in children in Alabama and Texas participating in social programs. Journal of the American Medical Association, 289, 1780 – 1781.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHarper, L.V., & Sanders, K.M. ( 1975 ). The effect of adults’ eating on young children’s acceptance of unfamiliar foods. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 20, 206 – 214.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHedley, A.A., Ogden, C.L., Johnson, C.L., Carroll, M.D., Curtin, L.R., & Flegal, K.M. ( 2004 ). Prevalence of overweight and obesity among US children, adolescents, and adults, 1999–2002. Journal of the American Medical Association, 291, 2847 – 2850.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHendy, H.M. ( 2002 ). Effectiveness of trained peer models to encourage food acceptance in preschool children. Appetite, 39, 217 – 225.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHendy, H.M., & Raudenbush, B. ( 2000 ). Effectiveness of teacher modeling to encourage food acceptance in preschool children. Appetite, 34, 61 – 76.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHeyes, C.M., & Galef, B.G. Jr. ( 1996 ). Social learning in animals: The roots of culture. New York: Academic Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJaswal, V.K., & Neely, L.A. ( 2006 ). Adults don’t always know best: preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words. Psychological Science, 17, 757 – 758.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKinzler, K.D., Shutts, K., & Correll, J. ( 2010 ). Priorities in social categories. European Journal of Social Psychology: Special Issue: Children’s Intergroup Attitudes, 40, 581 – 592.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.