Show simple item record

Do Lions Have Manes? For Children, Generics Are About Kinds Rather Than Quantities

dc.contributor.authorBrandone, Amanda C.en_US
dc.contributor.authorCimpian, Andreien_US
dc.contributor.authorLeslie, Sarah‐janeen_US
dc.contributor.authorGelman, Susan A.en_US
dc.date.accessioned2012-04-04T18:44:17Z
dc.date.available2013-05-01T17:24:44Zen_US
dc.date.issued2012-03en_US
dc.identifier.citationBrandone, Amanda C.; Cimpian, Andrei; Leslie, Sarah‐jane ; Gelman, Susan A. (2012). "Do Lions Have Manes? For Children, Generics Are About Kinds Rather Than Quantities." Child Development 83(2). <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/90604>en_US
dc.identifier.issn0009-3920en_US
dc.identifier.issn1467-8624en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/90604
dc.publisherBlackwell Publishing Ltden_US
dc.publisherWiley Periodicals, Inc.en_US
dc.titleDo Lions Have Manes? For Children, Generics Are About Kinds Rather Than Quantitiesen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollowen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelPsychologyen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumUniversity of Michiganen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherPrinceton Universityen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherUniversity of Illinois at Urbana‐Champaignen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherLehigh Universityen_US
dc.identifier.pmid22235892en_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/90604/1/j.1467-8624.2011.01708.x.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01708.xen_US
dc.identifier.sourceChild Developmenten_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePrasada, S. ( 2000 ). Acquiring generic knowledge. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 66 – 72.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKeil, F. C. ( 2006 ). Explanation and understanding. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 227 – 254.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKhemlani, S., Leslie, S. J., & Glucksberg, S. ( 2009 ). Generics, prevalence, and default inferences. Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKhemlani, S., Leslie, S. J., Glucksberg, S., & Rubio‐Fernandez, P. ( 2007 ). Do ducks lay eggs? How humans interpret generic assertions. Proceedings of the 29th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Nashville, TN: Cognitive Science Society.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLeslie, S. J. ( 2007 ). Generics and the structure of the mind. Philosophical Perspectives, 21, 375 – 405.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLeslie, S. J. ( 2008 ). Generics: Cognition and acquisition. Philosophical Review, 117, 1 – 49.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMarkman, E. M. ( 1989 ). Categorization and naming in children: Problems in induction. Cambridge: Bradford Book/MIT Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMurphy, G. L. ( 2002 ). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePiaget, J. ( 1952 ). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePrasada, S., & Dillingham, E. M. ( 2006 ). Principled and statistical connections in common sense conception. Cognition, 99, 73 – 112.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePrasada, S., & Dillingham, E. M. ( 2009 ). Representation of principled connections: A window onto the formal aspect of common sense conception. Cognitive Science, 33, 401 – 448.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceReynaert, C. C., & Gelman, S. A. ( 2007 ). The influence of language form and conventional wording on judgments of illness. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 272 – 295.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRips, L. J., & Collins, A. ( 1993 ). Categories and resemblance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 468 – 486.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSaffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. ( 1996 ). Statistical learning by 8‐month‐olds infants. Science, 274, 1926 – 1928.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceShipley, E. F. ( 1993 ). Categories, hierarchies, and induction. Psychology, Learning, and Motivation, 30, 265 – 301.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSloutsky, V. M., & Fisher, A. V. ( 2004 ). Induction and categorization in young children: A similarity‐based model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 166 – 188.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSloutsky, V. M., Kloos, H., & Fisher, A. V. ( 2007 ). When looks are everything: Appearance similarity versus kind information in early induction. Psychological Science, 18, 179 – 185.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSmith, L. B., Jones, S. S., & Landau, B. ( 1996 ). Naming in young children: A dumb attentional mechanism? Cognition, 60, 143 – 171.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSobel, D. M., & Kirkham, N. Z. ( 2007 ). Interactions between causal and statistical learning. In A. Gopnik & L. E. Schulz (Eds.), Causal learning: Psychology, philosophy, and computation (pp. 139 – 153 ). New York: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTenenbaum, J. B., Griffiths, T. L., & Kemp, C. ( 2006 ). Theory‐based Bayesian models of inductive learning and reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 311 – 318.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. ( 1998 ). Knowledge acquisition. In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language (4th ed., pp. 523 – 573 ). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAhn, W., Flanagan, E. H., Marsh, J. K., & Sanislow, C. A. ( 2006 ). Beliefs about essences and the reality of mental disorders. Psychological Science, 17, 759 – 766.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBrandone, A. C., & Gelman, S. A. ( 2009 ). Differences in preschoolers’ and adults’ use of generics about animals and artifacts: A window onto a conceptual divide. Cognition, 110, 1 – 22.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarey, S. ( 1985 ). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarey, S. ( 2009 ). The origin of concepts. New York: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarlson, G. N. ( 1977 ). A unified analysis of the English bare plural. Linguistics and Philosophy, 1, 412 – 456.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarlson, G. N., & Pelletier, F. J. (Eds.). ( 1995 ). The generic book. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChambers, C. G., Graham, S. A., & Turner, J. N. ( 2008 ). When hearsay trumps evidence: How generic language guides preschoolers’ inferences about unfamiliar things. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 749 – 766.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCimpian, A., Arce, H. C., Markman, E. M., & Dweck, C. S. ( 2007 ). Subtle linguistic cues affect children’s motivation. Psychological Science, 18, 314 – 316.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCimpian, A., Brandone, A. C., & Gelman, S. A. ( 2010 ). Generic statements require little evidence for acceptance but have powerful implications. Cognitive Science, 34, 1452 – 1482.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCimpian, A., Gelman, S. A., & Brandone, A. C. ( 2010 ). Theory‐based considerations influence the interpretation of generic sentences. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 261 – 276.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCimpian, A., & Markman, E. M. ( 2008 ). Preschool children’s use of cues to generic meaning. Cognition, 107, 19 – 53.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCimpian, A., Meltzer, T. J., & Markman, E. M. ( 2011 ). Preschoolers’ use of morphosyntactic cues to identify generic sentences: Indefinite singular noun phrases, tense, and aspect. Child Development, 82, 1561 – 1578.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceColunga, E., & Smith, L. B. ( 2005 ). From the lexicon to expectations about kinds: A role for associative learning. Psychological Review, 112, 347 – 382.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A. ( 2003 ). The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., & Bloom, P. ( 2007 ). Developmental changes in the understanding of generics. Cognition, 105, 166 – 183.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., & Brandone, A. C. ( 2010 ). Fast‐mapping placeholders: Using words to talk about kinds. Language Learning and Development, 6, 223 – 240.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., Coley, J. D., Rosengren, K., Hartman, E., & Pappas, A. ( 1998 ). Beyond labeling: The role of maternal input in the acquisition of richly‐structured categories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 62 No. 253).en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., Goetz, P. J., Sarnecka, B. S., & Flukes, J. ( 2008 ). Generic language in parent–child conversations. Language Learning and Development, 4, 1 – 31.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., & Heyman, G. D. ( 1999 ). Carrot‐eaters and creature‐believers: The effects of lexicalization on children’s inferences about social categories. Psychological Science, 10, 489 – 493.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., & Raman, L. ( 2003 ). Preschool children use linguistic form class and pragmatic cues to interpret generics. Child Development, 74, 308 – 325.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., Taylor, M. G., & Nguyen, S. ( 2004 ). Mother–child conversations about gender: Understanding the acquisition of essentialist beliefs. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69 No. 275).en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., Waxman, S., & Kleinberg, F. ( 2008 ). The role of representational status and item complexity in parent–child conversations about pictures and objects. Cognitive Development, 23, 313 – 323.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGilson, C., & Abelson, R. P. ( 1965 ). The subjective use of inductive evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 301 – 310.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. ( 1997 ). Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGopnik, A., & Schulz, L. ( 2004 ). Mechanisms of theory formation in young children. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 371 – 377.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGopnik, A., & Schulz, L. (Eds.). ( 2007 ). Causal learning: Psychology, philosophy, and computation. New York: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHollander, M. A., Gelman, S. A., & Star, J. ( 2002 ). Children’s interpretations of generic noun phrases. Developmental Psychology, 38, 883 – 894.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKeil, F. ( 1989 ). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Book/MIT Press.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.