Show simple item record

The Etiology of Individual Differences in Second Language Acquisition in Australian School Students: A Behavior‐Genetic Study

dc.contributor.authorCoventry, Williamen_US
dc.contributor.authorAntón‐méndez, Inésen_US
dc.contributor.authorEllis, Elizabeth M.en_US
dc.contributor.authorLevisen, Christinaen_US
dc.contributor.authorByrne, Brianen_US
dc.contributor.authorvan Daal, Victor H. P.en_US
dc.contributor.authorEllis, Nick C.en_US
dc.date.accessioned2012-09-05T14:46:11Z
dc.date.available2013-10-18T17:47:30Zen_US
dc.date.issued2012-09en_US
dc.identifier.citationCoventry, William; Antón‐méndez, Inés ; Ellis, Elizabeth M.; Levisen, Christina; Byrne, Brian; van Daal, Victor H. P.; Ellis, Nick C. (2012). "The Etiology of Individual Differences in Second Language Acquisition in Australian School Students: A Behaviorâ Genetic Study." Language Learning 62(3). <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/93553>en_US
dc.identifier.issn0023-8333en_US
dc.identifier.issn1467-9922en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/93553
dc.publisherBlackwell Publishing Incen_US
dc.publisherWiley Periodicals, Inc.en_US
dc.subject.otherTwinen_US
dc.subject.otherReading/Writingen_US
dc.subject.otherSecond Language Acquisitionen_US
dc.subject.otherBehavior Geneticsen_US
dc.subject.otherSpeaking/Listeningen_US
dc.titleThe Etiology of Individual Differences in Second Language Acquisition in Australian School Students: A Behavior‐Genetic Studyen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollowen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelLinguisticsen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelSocial Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationumUniversity of Michiganen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherUniversity of New Englanden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherUniversity of New Englanden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherUniversity of Stavangeren_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherUniversity of New England and Linköping Universityen_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherUniversity of New Englanden_US
dc.contributor.affiliationotherUniversity of New Englanden_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/93553/1/j.1467-9922.2012.00718.x.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/j.1467-9922.2012.00718.xen_US
dc.identifier.sourceLanguage Learningen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMiyake, A., & Friedman, N. ( 1998 ). Individual differences in second language proficiency: Working memory as language aptitude. In A. Healy & L. Bourne (Eds.), Foreign language learning: Psycholinguistic studies on training and retention (pp. 339 – 364 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEllis, R. ( 1997 ). SLA research and language teaching. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEllis, R. ( 2008 ). The study of second language acquisition ( 2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceElman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff‐Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. ( 1996 ). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFalconer, D. S. ( 1960 ). Introduction to quantitative genetics. New York: Ronald Press Company.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceField, A. ( 2009 ). Discovering statistics using SPSS ( 3rd ed.). Los Angeles: Sage.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFriedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. ( 2008 ). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 201 – 225.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGrigorenko, E. L. ( 2007 ). How can genomics inform education Mind, Brain, and Education, 1, 20 – 27.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHart, S. A., Petrill, S. A., Thompson, L. A., & Plomin, R. ( 2009 ). The ABCs of math: A genetic analysis of mathematics and its links with reading ability and general cognitive ability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 388 – 402.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHoge, R. D., & Colardaci, T. ( 1989 ). Teacher‐based judgments of academic achievement: A review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 59, 297 – 313.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceJuffs, A., & Harrington, M. ( 2011 ). Aspects of working memory in L2 learning. Language Teaching, 44, 137 – 166.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J. ( 1994 ). Parental treatment and the equal environments assumption in twin studies of psychiatric illness. Psychological Medicine, 24, 579 – 590.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKovas, Y., Haworth, C. M. A., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. ( 2007 ). The genetic and environmental origins of learning abilities and disabilities in the early school years. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 72, 1 – 160.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKovas, Y., & Plomin, R. ( 2006 ). Generalist genes: Implications for the cognitive sciences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 198 – 203.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLiberman, I. Y., & Liberman, A. M. ( 1992 ). Whole language versus code emphasis: Underlying assumptions and their implications for reading instruction. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 343 – 366 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMcCall, R. B., Evahn, C., & Kratzer, L. ( 1992 ). High school underachievers: What do they achieve as adults? Pittsburgh, PA: Sage.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMonaco, A. P. ( 2007 ). Multivariate linkage analysis of Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Annals of Human Genetics, 71, 660 – 673.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNeale, M. C. ( 2004 ). Mx: Statistical modelling (Version 1.5.01). Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry, Medical College of Virginia.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNichols, R. C., & Bilbro, W. C. ( 1966 ). The diagnosis of twin zygosity. Acta Genetica, 16, 265 – 275.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceOliver, B., Harlaar, N., Hayiou, T., Marianna, E., Kovas, Y., Walker, S. O., et al. ( 2004 ). A twin study of teacher‐reported mathematics performance and low performance in 7‐year‐olds. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 504 – 517.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePinker, S. ( 1994 ). The language instinct. London: Allen Lane.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePlomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E., & McGuffin, P. ( 2008 ). Behavioral genetics ( 5 th ed. ). New York: Worth.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferencePlomin, R., Kovas, Y., & Haworth, C. M. A. ( 2007 ). Generalist genes: Genetic links between brain, mind, and education. Mind, Brain, and Education, 1, 11 – 19.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRichardson, K., & Norgate, S. ( 2005 ). The equal environments assumption of classical twin studies may not hold. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 339 – 359.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSamuelsson, S., Byrne, B., Olson, R. K., Hulslander, J., Wadsworth, S., Corley, R., et al. ( 2008 ). Response to early literacy instruction in the United States, Australia, and Scandinavia: A behavioral‐genetic analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 289 – 295.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSamuelsson, S., Byrne, B., Quain, P., Wadsworth, S., Corley, R., DeFries, J. C., et al. ( 2005 ). Environmental and genetic influences on prereading skills in Australia, Scandinavia, and the United States. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 705 – 722.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSawyer, M., & Ranta, L. ( 2001 ). Aptitude, individual differences, and instructional design. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 319 – 353 ). New York: Cambridge University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSlutske, W. S., Heath, A. C., Dinwiddie, S. H., Madden, P. A. F., Bucholz, K. K., Dunne, M. P., et al. ( 1997 ). Modeling genetic and environmental influences in the etiology of conduct disorder: A study of 2,682 adult twin pairs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 266 – 279.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. ( 1996 ). Using multivariate statistics ( 3rd ed. ). New York: HarperCollins.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTaylor, J., Roehrig, A. D., Soden Hensler, B., Connor, C. M., & Schatschneider, C. ( 2010 ). Teacher quality moderates the genetic effects on early reading. Science, 328, 512 – 514.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWainwright, M. A., Wright, M., Luciano, M., Geffen, G., & Martin, N. ( 2005 ). Multivariate genetic analysis of academic skills of the Queensland Core Skills Test and IQ highlight the importance of genetic g. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 8, 602 – 608.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceVinkhuyzen, A. A. E., van der Sluis, S., Posthuma, D., & Boomsma, D. I. ( 2009 ). The heritability of aptitude and expectional talent across different domains in adolescents and young adults. Behavior Genetics, 39, 380 – 392.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAlvidrez, J., & Weinstein, R. S. ( 1999 ). Early teacher perceptions and later student achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 731 – 746.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAndo, J. ( 1992 ). The effects of two EFL (English as a foreign language) teaching approaches studied by the cotwin control method: A comparative study of the communicative and the grammatical approaches. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae: Twin Research, 41, 335 – 352.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBaddeley, A., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. ( 1998 ). The phonological look as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158 – 173.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBrkanac, Z., Chapman, N. H., Igo, R. P. J., Matsushita, M. M., Nielsen, K., Berninger, V. W., et al. ( 2008 ). Genome scan of a nonword repetition phenotype in families with dyslexia: Evidence for multiple loci. Behavioral Genetics, 38, 462 – 475.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceByrne, B. ( 2011 ). Evaluating the role of phonological factors in early literacy development: Insights from experimental and behavior‐genetic studies. In S. Brady, D. Braze, & C. Fowler (Eds.), The phonological deficit hypothesis (pp. 175 – 196 ). New York: Taylor and Francis.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceByrne, B., Coventry, W. L., Olson, R. K., Samuelsson, S., Corley, R., Willcutt, E. G., et al. ( 2009 ). Genetic and environmental influences on aspects of literacy and language in early childhood: Continuity and change from preschool to Grade 2. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 22, 219 – 236.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceByrne, B., Coventry, W. L., Olson, R. K., Wadsworth, S. J., Samuelsson, S., Petrill, S. A., et al. ( 2010 ). “Teacher effects” in early literacy development: Evidence from a study of twins. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 32 – 42.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceByrne, B., Khlentzos, D., Olson, R. K., & Samuelsson, S. ( 2010 ). Evolutionary and genetic perspectives on educational attainment. In K. Littleton, C. Wood, & J. K. Staarman (Eds.), International handbook of psychology in education (pp. 3 – 34 ). Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceByrne, B., & Liberman, A. M. ( 1999 ). Meaninglessness, productivity and reading: Some observations about the relationship between the alphabet and speech. In J. Oakhill & R. Beard (Eds.), Reading development and the teaching of reading: A psychological perspective (pp. 157 – 173 ). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceByrne, B., Wadsworth, S., Boehme, K., Talk, A., Coventry, W., Olson, R. K., et al. (in press). How general are the genes influencing learning to read words? Scientific Studies of Reading.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceByrne, B., Wadsworth, S., Corley, R., Samuelsson, S., Quain, P., DeFries, J. C., et al. ( 2005 ). Longitudinal twin study of early literacy development: Preschool and kindergarten phases. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 219 – 235.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCenoz, J. ( 2003 ). The additive effect on third language acquisition: A review. International Journal of Bilingualism, 7, 71 – 88.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceChristiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. ( 2008 ). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 489 – 558.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCohen, J. ( 1960 ). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 37 – 46.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDale, P. S., Harlaar, N., Haworth, C. M. A., & Plomin, R. ( 2010 ). Two by two: A twin study of second‐language acquisition. Psychological Science, 21, 635 – 640.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDavis, O. S. P., Kovas, Y., Harlaar, N., Busfield, P., McMillan, A., Frances, J., et al. ( 2008 ). Generalist genes and the Internet generation: Etiology of learning abilities by web testing at age 10. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 7, 455 – 462.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceDerks, E. M., Dolan, C. V., & Boomsma, D. I. ( 2006 ). A test of the equal environments assumption (EEA) in multivariate twin studies. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 403 – 411.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEllis, E. M. ( 2004 ). The invisible multilingual teacher: The contribution of language background to Australian ESL teachers’ professional knowledge and beliefs. International Journal of Multilingualism, 1, 90 – 108.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceEllis, N. C. ( 2005 ). At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 305 – 352.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.