Show simple item record

Tracking the Actions and Possessions of Agents

dc.contributor.authorGelman, Susan A.en_US
dc.contributor.authorNoles, Nicholaus S.en_US
dc.contributor.authorStilwell, Sarahen_US
dc.date.accessioned2014-11-04T16:35:47Z
dc.date.availableWITHHELD_12_MONTHSen_US
dc.date.available2014-11-04T16:35:47Z
dc.date.issued2014-10en_US
dc.identifier.citationGelman, Susan A.; Noles, Nicholaus S.; Stilwell, Sarah (2014). "Tracking the Actions and Possessions of Agents." Topics in Cognitive Science 6(4): 599-614.en_US
dc.identifier.issn1756-8757en_US
dc.identifier.issn1756-8765en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/109337
dc.description.abstractWe propose that there is a powerful human disposition to track the actions and possessions of agents. In two experiments, 3‐year‐olds and adults viewed sets of objects, learned a new fact about one of the objects in each set (either that it belonged to the participant, or that it possessed a particular label), and were queried about either the taught fact or an unrelated dimension (preference) immediately after a spatiotemporal transformation, and after a delay. Adults uniformly tracked object identity under all conditions, whereas children tracked identity more when taught ownership versus labeling information, and only regarding the taught fact (not the unrelated dimension). These findings suggest that the special attention that children and adults pay to agents readily extends to include inanimate objects. That young children track an object's history, despite their reliance on surface features on many cognitive tasks, suggests that unobservable historical features are foundational in human cognition.en_US
dc.publisherOxford University Pressen_US
dc.publisherWiley Periodicals, Inc.en_US
dc.subject.otherChildrenen_US
dc.subject.otherArtifactsen_US
dc.subject.otherConceptsen_US
dc.subject.otherEssentialismen_US
dc.subject.otherLabelingen_US
dc.subject.otherOwnershipen_US
dc.subject.otherObject Trackingen_US
dc.titleTracking the Actions and Possessions of Agentsen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.rights.robotsIndexNoFollowen_US
dc.subject.hlbsecondlevelNeurology and Neurosciencesen_US
dc.subject.hlbtoplevelHealth Sciencesen_US
dc.description.peerreviewedPeer Revieweden_US
dc.description.bitstreamurlhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/109337/1/tops12106.pdf
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/tops.12106en_US
dc.identifier.sourceTopics in Cognitive Scienceen_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMeltzoff, A. N. ( 1995 ). Understanding the intentions of others: Re‐enactment of intended acts by 18‐month‐old children. Developmental Psychology, 31 ( 5 ), 838 – 850.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHall, D. ( 1996 ). Preschoolers' default assumptions about word meaning: Proper names designate unique individuals. Developmental Psychology, 32 ( 1 ), 177 – 186.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. ( 2007 ). Social evaluation in preverbal infants. Nature, 450 ( 7169 ), 557 – 559.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceHarbaugh, W. T., Krause, K., & Vesterlund, L. ( 2001 ). Are adults better behaved than children? Age, experience, and the endowment effect. Economics Letters, 70, 175 – 181.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. H. ( 1990 ). Experimental tests of the endowment effect and the Coase Theorem. Journal of Political Economy, 98, 1325 – 1348.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKalish, C. W. ( 1996 ). Preschoolers' understanding of germs as invisible mechanisms. Cognitive Development, 11 ( 1 ), 83 – 106.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKanngiesser, P., Gjersoe, N., & Hood, B. M. ( 2010 ). The effect of creative labor on property‐ownership transfer by preschool children and adults. Psychological Science, 21 ( 9 ), 1236 – 1241.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceKim, S., & Kalish, C. W. ( 2009 ). Children's ascriptions of property rights with changes of ownership. Cognitive Development, 24 ( 3 ), 322 – 336.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLee, C., Linkenauger, S. A., Bakdash, J. Z., Joy‐Gaba, J. A., & Profitt, D. R. ( 2011 ). Putting like a pro: The role of positive contagion in golf performance and perception. PLoS ONE, 6 ( 10 ).en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLegare, C., Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. ( 2009 ). Evidence for an explanation advantage in naïve biological reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 177 – 194.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceLyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. ( 2007 ). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 19751 – 19756.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceMarkson, L., & Bloom, P. ( 1997 ). Evidence against a dedicated system for word learning in children. Nature, 385 ( 6619 ), 813 – 815.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceNoles, N. S., Keil, F. C., Bloom, P., & Gelman, S. A. ( 2012 ). Children's and adults' intuitions about who can own things. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 12 ( 3–4 ), 265 – 286.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRosen, A., & Rozin, P. ( 1993 ). Now you see it..Now you don't: The preschool child's conception of invisible particles in the context of dissolving. Developmental Psychology, 29, 300 – 311.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRoss, J., Anderson, J. R., & Campbell, R. N. ( 2011 ). I remember me: Mnemonic self‐reference effects in preschool children: I. Why investigate mnemonic self‐reference effects in preschoolers? Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76 ( 3 ), 1 – 16.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceRozin, P., & Nemeroff, C. J. ( 1990 ). The laws of sympathetic magic: A psychological analysis of similarity and contagion. In J. Stigler, G. Herdt, & R. A. Shweder (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human development (pp. 205 – 232 ). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSiegal, M., & Share, D. L. ( 1990 ). Contamination sensitivity in young children. Developmental Psychology, 26 ( 3 ), 455 – 458.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSorrentino, C. M. ( 2001 ). Children and adults represent proper names as referring to unique individuals. Developmental Science, 4, 399 – 407.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceSpelke, E. ( 1995 ). Initial knowledge: Six suggestions. In J. Mehler & S. Franck (Eds.), Cognition on cognition (pp. 433 – 447 ). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTaylor, M. G., Rhodes, M., & Gelman, S. A. ( 2009 ). Boys will be boys; cows will be cows: Children's essentialist reasoning about gender categories and animal species. Child Development, 79, 1270 – 1287.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceTurk, D. J., van Bussel, K., Waiter, G. D., & Macrae, C. ( 2011 ). Mine and me: Exploring the neural basis of object ownership. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23 ( 11 ), 3657 – 3668.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWaxman, S. R., & Booth, A. E. ( 2000 ). Principles that are invoked in the acquisition of words, but not facts. Cognition, 77 ( 2 ), B33 – B43.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceWoodward, A. L. ( 1998 ). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition, 69, 1 – 34.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceXu, F., & Carey, S. ( 1996 ). Infants' metaphysics: The case of numerical identity. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 111 – 153.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceAu, T. K., Sidle, A. L., & Rollins, K. B. ( 1993 ). Developing an intuitive understanding of conservation and contamination: Invisible particles as a plausible mechanism. Developmental Psychology, 29, 286 – 299.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBeggan, J. K. ( 1992 ). On the social nature of nonsocial perception: The mere ownership effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62 ( 2 ), 229 – 237.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBehrend, D. A., Scofield, J., & Kleinknecht, E. E. ( 2001 ). Beyond fast mapping: Young children's extensions of novel words and novel facts. Developmental Psychology, 37 ( 5 ), 698 – 705.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBlake, P. R., & Harris, P. L. ( 2009 ). Children's understanding of ownership transfers. Cognitive Development, 24 ( 2 ), 133 – 145.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBloom, P. ( 1996 ). Intention, history, and artifact concepts. Cognition, 60, 1 – 29.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBullot, N. J., & Reber, R. ( 2013 ). The artful mind meets art history: Toward a psycho‐historical framework for the science of art appreciation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 123 – 137.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceBuresh, J., & Woodward, A. L. ( 2007 ). Infants track action goals within and across agents. Cognition, 104 ( 2 ), 287 – 314.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarey, S. ( 2009 ). The origin of concepts. New York: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCarey, S., & Bartlett, E. ( 1978 ). Acquiring a single new word. Proceedings of the Stanford Child Language Conference, 15, 17 – 29.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCsibra, G., & Gergely, G. ( 2009 ). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 148 – 153.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceCunningham, S., Vergunst, F., Macrae, C. N., & Turk, D. J. ( 2013 ). Exploring early self‐referential memory effects through ownership. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31, 289 – 301.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceFriedman, O., & Neary, K. R. ( 2008 ). Determining who owns what: Do children infer ownership from first possession? Cognition, 107 ( 3 ), 829 – 849.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A. ( 2003 ). The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. New York: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., Manczak, E. M., & Noles, N. S. ( 2012 ). The non‐obvious basis of ownership: Preschool children trace the history and value of owned objects. Child Development, 83 ( 5 ), 1732 – 1747.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGelman, S. A., Meyer, M. A., & Noles, N. S. ( 2013 ). History and essence in human cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 142 – 143.en_US
dc.identifier.citedreferenceGutheil, G., Gelman, S. A., Klein, E., Michos, K., & Kelaita, K. ( 2008 ). Preschoolers' use of spatiotemporal history, appearance, and proper name in determining individual identity. Cognition, 107, 366 – 380.en_US
dc.owningcollnameInterdisciplinary and Peer-Reviewed


Files in this item

Show simple item record

Remediation of Harmful Language

The University of Michigan Library aims to describe library materials in a way that respects the people and communities who create, use, and are represented in our collections. Report harmful or offensive language in catalog records, finding aids, or elsewhere in our collections anonymously through our metadata feedback form. More information at Remediation of Harmful Language.

Accessibility

If you are unable to use this file in its current format, please select the Contact Us link and we can modify it to make it more accessible to you.